Irish Gifted Students: Self, Social, and Academic Explorations
Jennifer Riedl Cross, Ph.D.
Tracy L. Cross, Ph.D.
William & Mary Center for Gifted Education
Colm O’Reilly, Ph.D.
Centre for Talented Youth - Ireland
Acknowledgements
Over the past decade, many individuals were critical to the success of this project. Numerous members of the CTYI staff were invaluable for their help in gathering and entering data. Dr. Leeanne Hinch coordinated the administration of surveys and interviews in multiple studies. Rebecca McDonnell assisted in the analysis of the pandemic study. Dr. Catriona Ledwith coordinated the final edits for publication. We would also like to thank the Faculties and Senior Management in DCU for their continued support of the work of CTYI. In the US, Dr. Tom Ward provided support for the statistical analyses. Colin Vaughn, Sakhavat Mammadov, and Anyesha Mishra managed and analyzed data in multiple studies. Marialena Kostouli at the Center for Talented Youth-Greece and Dr. Paromita Roy at the Jagadis Bose National Science Center in Kolkata, India spent countless hours collecting data from students in their programs. The authors wish to thank all those who contributed their time and expertise to enhance our understanding of Irish gifted students.
Executive Summary
In 2011, Dr. Colm O’Reilly, the Director of the Irish Centre for Talented Youth (CTYI), and Dr. Tracy L. Cross, the Executive Director of the William & Mary Center for Gifted Education (CFGE) developed a partnership to conduct research with or on behalf of gifted students in Ireland. Over the next ten years, numerous studies were conducted to learn about these students and about gifted education in the country via educators’ and parents’ beliefs and experiences. Two reports have been published on the former: Gifted Education in Ireland: Educators’ Beliefs and Practices and Gifted Education in Ireland: Parents’ Beliefs and Experiences, both available from CTYI. This report describes the findings of research conducted with CTYI students for the purpose of supporting the well-being and maximization of potential among Irish gifted students. It is divided into four chapters
Chapter 1: Introduction – A description of the studies and the participating students
Chapter 6: Recommendations & Conclusions
The Studies
Ten studies were conducted with more than 2600 students attending CTYI programs, two with students in Greece and India. Nearly all participants were secondary students and 46% were female. Three studies were interviews and the remaining used questionnaires. Most students (44%) were from county Dublin, but every Irish county had some students represented. All other students scored at the 95th percentile and above.
The Psychology of Irish Gifted Students
The majority of CTYI secondary students (66%) had resilient personalities – they were sociable, agreeable, conscientious, emotionally stable, and open to new experiences. Nearly all students exhibited high levels of confidence in their academic abilities and most had confidence in all academic and social domains. About a third of students had potential risk factors indicating additional supports may be needed. These personality differences provide a framework for later analysis of students’ social and academic experiences.
The Social Experience of Irish Gifted Students
In several studies, CTYI students confirmed the findings from previous research that their exceptional abilities can lead to challenges in their relationships with others. They reported experiences of hiding their abilities and conforming to others’ behaviors to maintain positive relationships with peers. Their abilities were often visible to peers and being known as an advanced student was generally a positive experience. The frequent pressure to achieve and always be right was not as positive. Expressing one’s gifted abilities could sometimes be a costly experience and some CTYI students preferred to lie over telling the truth in situations when their abilities might be exposed. Painful peer rejection occurred for some CTYI students, but most did not consider themselves to be ostracized. They preferred to work independently and considered themselves more serious about learning than peers. Being able to help peers with their exceptional abilities was positive, but older students sometimes felt the expectation to help was burdensome. CTYI programs gave them a welcome chance to spend time with intellectual peers whose high levels of interest in learning were similar to theirs.
During the COVID-19 pandemic, online school inhibited social connections, when peers withdrew behind muted cameras and microphones and there was little opportunity to interact in classes. This atmosphere had one advantage: bullying was not possible when there was no face-to-face interaction.
Students were positive about their family relationships and most students were confident they could get support from their parents to solve social or academic problems. About a quarter of students were less confident in their parents’ support. Positive attitudes toward school were correlated with students’ positive relationships with their parents.
The Academic Experience of Irish Gifted Students
An appropriate education is important not only for students’ psychological well-being, but also for the maximization of their potential. CTYI students are capable of learning at an advanced level in some or all subjects. About half of them were confident in their abilities in all subject areas, but others had greater confidence in their abilities in either math, science, or humanities-related subject areas. In school, most CTYI students reported they rarely or never received differentiated lessons targeted at their ability level. They were often bored by lessons because they already knew the material. In interviews, students described a difficult learning environment, often focused on the needs of the typical student, who learned less rapidly and was less serious about their learning. CTYI students considered good teachers to be those with high expectations, who were enthusiastic and knowledgeable about their subjects, and had effective teaching strategies. While they may have had good teachers, they also gave many examples of times when they were not learning. Students readily shared their opinions about CTYI programs offering exciting opportunities for challenge in stimulating subjects.
Compared to in-person school, online school during the COVID-19 pandemic offered less support from teachers, was less motivating, and presented difficulties in managing their own learning. The majority of students were pleased to be back in their home school. CTYI’s online classes were perceived by students to be much more motivating and CTYI teachers were perceived to be more supportive than those in their online school.
International Comparisons
Partners at the Center for Talented Youth-Greece (CTYG), at Anatolia College in Thessalonika, and the Jagadis Bose National Science Talent Search (JBNS) in Kolkata conducted studies to parallel a study with CTYI and CAT students. There were many more similarities than differences among the students in psychological comparisons. Socially, all students agreed they were more serious about learning than peers and preferred to work independently. Both CTYG and JBNS students appeared less concerned about hiding their ability from peers than CTYI or CAT students. In academic comparisons, JBNS students reported receiving more regularly differentiated assignments than the other students. While the amount of boredom differed by subject for each country, students in all programs reported being bored once a week or more often in some of their classes.
Conclusion
CTYI students represent a unique population, with social and academic experiences their peers do not share. While most CTYI students have positive, even exceptionally positive, psychological profiles, some students will require support for optimal well-being and, ultimately, achievement of their potential. Adults who work with and care for CTYI students should be aware of the social challenges presented by their abilities and the need to provide an appropriate curriculum, delivered at an appropriate pace. A talent development approach would be an inclusive, effective framework for gifted education in Ireland.
Table of Contents
Chapter 1
There has been interest in the education of exceptionally capable students for centuries. Testing has long played an important role in finding this potential, from the Imperial Examinations to identify civil servants during the Han Dynasty (206 BCE-220 CE) in China (Zhang, 2017) to the IQ tests used by Lewis Terman (1925) in his study of 1000 “geniuses.”
The Centre for Talented Youth-Ireland (CTYI) continues this tradition by utilizing standardized tests to find primary and secondary students who perform at the 95th percentile and above. These students are often not well served by school systems that focus on the development of average ability students, as is generally the case across Ireland (O’Reilly, 2013). Founded in 1992 based on the model of the Center for Talented Youth at Johns Hopkins University, CTYI has grown significantly over the past 30 years. It has served thousands of highability Irish students by offering enrichment courses that expose students to topics not covered in schools, allowing in-depth exploration. A fee-based program, CTYI has expanded its offerings to low-income students through scholarships and grant-funded courses. The Centre for Academic Talent (CAT) program offers courses for students whose test scores fall between the 85th and 94th percentile, opening CTYI opportunities to an even wider swath of highly capable Irish students. The only centre for gifted education in Ireland, CTYI provides an important educational and advocacy function.
In the fall of 2010, the directors of the CTYI and the William & Mary Center for Gifted Education (CFGE) began a conversation that developed into a strong relationship between the two organizations. The mutual desire to support the needs of gifted students led to numerous collaborative research projects, publications, and presentations around the world. Previous reports have highlighted the beliefs and experiences of educators and parents (J. Cross et al., 2014, 2019). In this report, we will describe the findings of the ten studies with CTYI students and two studies with international students conducted between 2012 and 2021. Table 1.1 includes a list of the studies and Tables 1.2 and 1.3 describe participating student demographics.
Prior to 2012, very few studies had been published about Irish gifted students. In fact, only one study could be found that related to their psychology. In the mid-1990s, Mills and Parker (1998) studied students attending the new CTYI program and compared them with U.S. students participating in the Center for Talented Youth program at Johns Hopkins University. Much more is known about the psychology of gifted students in the US. Research with U.S. samples has considered their mental health (Cross & Cross, 2015; Martin et al., 2010), personality (Mammadov, 2021; Vuyk et al., 2016), self-concept (Dai & Rinn, 2008; Rinn et al., 2010), perfectionistic attitudes (Fletcher & Speirs Neumeister, 2012), achievement goal orientation (Speirs Neumeister, 2004), peer relationships (J. Cross, 2021; T. Cross & Cross, 2022), and attitudes toward their giftedness (Berlin, 2009). This research has led to a focus on the social and emotional needs of gifted students, along with recommendations for practice
One line of research began with Coleman (1985), who proposed that gifted students may encounter a stigma in society that interferes with their ability to be accepted and to develop normally. Coleman’s stigma of giftedness paradigm (SGP) has three tenets:
-
Gifted students, like all students, desire normal interactions with their classmates;
-
as others learn of their giftedness, they will be treated differently; and
-
gifted students can increase their social latitude by managing the information others have of them.
Researchers found that gifted students did, indeed, sometimes attempt to hide their abilities from peers (T. Cross et al., 1991; Swiatek, 2012). The potential of such behaviors to impact students’ psychological, social, and academic development makes this a valuable endeavor. In their influential monograph, Subotnik and colleagues (2011) stress the importance of psychosocial variables in talent development. “Psychosocial variables are determining factors in the successful development of talent” (p. 7), they claim, citing copious research as evidence.
Our primary goal in this research project has been to support the well-being and maximization of potential among Irish gifted students. By learning more about them and their experiences, we hope to provide a foundation on which to build this support in their homes and schools. The questions driving the research in this collaboration emphasized three topics in relation to Irish gifted students:
-
Their psychology, in particular, their self-beliefs.
-
Their social experience
-
Their school experience
The research has been approached through both quantitative and qualitative methodologies, allowing for a broad perspective on students’ psychology and experiences. Over the years, researchers in other talent search or gifted education programs have become interested in this project. As a result, we are able to draw comparisons with high-ability students in not only the US, but also South Korea, France, the United Kingdom, Greece, and India.
Year | Level | No. of Participants | Method | Constructs Included |
---|---|---|---|---|
2012 | Primary & Secondary | 374 | Survey | Self-Concept (SDQI); Social Coping, Social Dominance Orientation |
2013a | Primary & Secondary | 18 | Interviews | Social Experience of Giftedness |
2013b | Secondary | 295 | Survey | Implicit Theory, Ostracism, Selfefficacy, Self-Concept |
2014 | Secondary | 163 | Survey | Self-efficacy, Ostracism, Personality |
2015 | Secondary | 494 | Survey | Social Cognitive Beliefs Scale, Class challenge/depth, Personality, Self-efficacy, Perfectionism, Ostracism, Implicit Theory |
2016 | Secondary-CAT | 351 | Survey | Social Cognitive Beliefs Scale, Class challenge/depth, Personality, Self-efficacy, Perfectionism, Ostracism, Implicit Theory |
2017 | International-India | 457 | Survey | Social Cognitive Beliefs Scale, Class challenge/depth, Personality, Self-efficacy, Ostracism, Implicit Theory |
2017 | International-Greece | 146 | Survey | Social Cognitive Beliefs Scale, Class challenge/ depth, Self-efficacy, Ostracism, Implicit Theory |
2018 | Secondary | 559 | Survey | Social Experience Scale, Personality |
2019 | Secondary | 12 | Interviews | School Experience |
2021a | Secondary | 326 | Survey | Pandemic Academic Experience |
2021b | Secondary | 16 | Interviews | Pandemic Social Experience |
Between 2012 and 2021, the students described in Table 1.1 participated in surveys and interviews. In all survey studies, student anonymity was preserved, with no identifying information collected. Data collected via interviews preserves students’ confidentiality. Data was quite evenly distributed between males and females. To reflect changing societal recognition of gender fluidity, additional gender options were included in the surveys from 2018 on. Surveys of primary students were conducted only in 2012. The 2016 students surveyed were in the Centre for Academic Talent (CAT) program. These students scored between the 85th and 94th percentile on standardized achievement tests. All other students scored at the 95th percentile and above. In 2015, 2016, and 2021, students were asked to identify their home counties. Nearly all Irish counties, including several in Northern Ireland, were represented (see map in Figure 1.1). The majority of students were from County Dublin.
Interviews were conducted with students in 2013, 2019, and 2021. The 2013 interviews were part of a five country cross-cultural study of the social experience of gifted students (J. Cross, Vaughn et al., 2019). In each country, three male and three female students at the elementary (4th and 5th Class), middle (2nd Year), and high school (4th and 5th Year) levels were interviewed, totaling 18 students. In 2019, six male and six female secondary level students (2nd through 6th Year) were interviewed about their school experiences. The full report goes into detail with our findings, making the most of these students’ time and openness. In this brief report, we will summarize the findings of the decades-long study of students participating in CTYI programs. It is our hope that this research is of benefit to Irish gifted students and their counterparts around the world.
Chapter 2
One of the primary objectives of this research project has been to support the well-being of Irish gifted students. According to the dictionary of the American Psychological Association (APA), well-being is defined as “a state of happiness and contentment, with low levels of distress, overall good physical and mental health and outlook, or good quality of life” (APA, 2020). Well-being has rarely been studied among gifted students, but some studies have explored psychological constructs that lead to the opposite – high levels of distress – in this population (J. Cross & Cross, 2015). For example, there appears to be no difference in rates of depression among academically gifted students compared to their nongifted peers (Martin et al., 2010), although rates of depression have been found to be higher among creatively gifted individuals (Neihart & Olenchak, 2002). An analysis of four studies found levels of anxiety to be lower among gifted students than non-gifted peers (Martin et al., 2010). Studies of suicidal ideation (thinking about killing oneself) among gifted students find no difference from comparable samples in the general population (T. Cross & Cross, 2017). Depression, anxiety, suicidal ideation – these negative psychological conditions are linked in research in the general population with personality differences (Hakulinen et al., 2015; Lyon et al., 2021), self-concept (Matthews, 2014), perfectionism (Smith et al., 2016), self-efficacy (Bandura, 2001; Maddux, 1995), and even beliefs about the malleability of intelligence or personality (Schroder et al., 2015). To best support Irish gifted students’ well-being, we need to have a picture of their psychological make-up.
In the quantitative studies listed in Table 1.1, we asked students to share their beliefs about- what they are like (self-concept [2012, 2013], personality [2015, 2016]); what they can do (self-efficacy [2013, 2014, 2015, 2016]); how perfect they need to be, for themselves or others (perfectionism [2015, 2016]); whether people can change their intelligence or personality (implicit theory [2013, 2015, 2016]); and what they believe about how resources should be distributed in society (social dominance orientation [2012]). This section will describe what we learned about the CTYI students who participated in these studies. We can infer from these data what steps may be best to take to support students who may be vulnerable to negative psychological outcomes.
The most important lesson from our psychological research with CTYI students is that they are not a monolith. There is not one profile of an Irish gifted student that fits them all. This may seem obvious, but much previous research has attempted to explain the essence of a gifted student. By aggregating data, we can come up with an average profile, but such an average can be quite misleading. In his book, The End of Average, author Todd Rose (2016) described the efforts of the U.S. air force to create a cockpit that fit all pilots by using the average measurements of 4,000 pilots on 10 dimensions, such as arm and leg length, chest circumference, and so forth. After identifying the average, they discovered that not a single pilot was exactly average and fewer than 3.5% matched on just three dimensions. Keeping this lesson in mind, where possible, we have attempted to explore the data from a person-centered perspective. We first apply analyses in the aggregate, but then go deeper to examine clusters or classes of students who fit various profiles.
When we ask the question, “What is a person like?” there are many ways they can be described. We can describe their physical appearance, their abilities, their motivations, their patterns of behavior, or any number of other characteristics. Every individual is unique, but we often seek to find similarities that help us in making sense of others. Their personality, or their characteristic patterns of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, are of particular importance to this sense-making.
In recent decades, personality research has consistently identified five dimensions: Openness to new experience, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism (OCEAN, the common mnemonic). These dimensions exist on a continuum, from open to closed to new experiences; from highly conscientious to disorganized and lacking in discipline; from outgoing (extravert) to reticent (introvert); from agreeable to disagreeable; and from emotionally stable to unstable (neurotic). Individuals will differ from others by degree on each dimension.
In personality comparisons, CTYI and CAT students from the 2015 and 2016 studies were less extraverted (outgoing) and more conscientious than the general population represented in the large international norm group. CTYI students were more introverted and less agreeable than CAT students, who tended to be more agreeable and less emotionally unstable (neurotic) than the general population. Numerous studies have found gifted students to have a stronger tendency toward introversion than the general population and this was confirmed in the CTYI and CAT samples.
Numerous studies have found three consistent profiles in personality characteristics:
-
Resilient – low in neuroticism (emotional instability), high in other traits
-
Overcontroller – high in neuroticism (emotional instability), low in extraversion (introvert)
-
Undercontroller – high in extraversion, low in agreeableness, low in conscientiousness
Resilients are flexible and adapt well to changing situations, leading them to generally be well-adjusted. Overcontrollers, who were so named because of their strong tendency to control the expression of their emotional and motivational impulses, have been found to be more likely to experience internalizing symptoms, such as depression and anxiety. Undercontrollers have the opposite tendency – they do not try to control their impulses. This personality pattern is associated with externalizing problems such as aggression, hyperactivity, and acting out.
The personality profiles among CTYI students in the 2015 study were mostly consistent with these patterns, but with important differences. Instead of having three profiles, the CTYI students had four, with not one resilient class, but two: moderate and high Resilients. Both classes of Resilients were high in the characteristics associated with positive adjustment – sociable, agreeable, conscientious, emotionally stable, and open to new experiences – but the High Resilients were very high in these characteristics. Together, the two resilient classes make up two-thirds (66.3%) of the CTYI sample. The CTYI Undercontroller class, 9.6% of the sample, was different from those identified in the literature, as well. Like those found in the general population, this group was highly extraverted, lowest of the CTYI students in agreeableness, but with a moderate level of conscientiousness, which is likely how they met the testing requirement for entry to CTYI. The majority of Overcontrollers were female (63.8%) and the majority of Moderate Resilients were male (63.5%).
CTYI students shared their beliefs about other aspects of their lives as well. The instrument used to measure students’ self-concept was the Self-Description Questionnaire I, which measured student perceptions of their Academic, Non-Academic, and General Selves. A majority of CTYI students in the 2012 study had overall high (n = 156; 44.2%) or moderately high (n = 86; 24.4%) levels of self-concept, with positive beliefs about their non-academic, academic, and general selves. Highest scores were seen among primary students and nearly all students had high reading self-concepts. Among secondary students, males had higher self-concept scores than females in most areas, with the exception of reading, parent relations, and general school.
What CTYI students will pursue as they develop will depend in part on what they believe about themselves. Self-concept is one’s perceptions of who they are, what they are interested in, and how they evaluate themselves: “Who am I?” “What do I like/dislike?” “Am I good/not good at ___?” These beliefs will likely have an impact on their academic pursuits. Another aspect of CTYI students’ psychology is their self-efficacy: their perceptions of their capability to carry out an activity (Bandura, 1986). In other words, self-efficacy is a measure of confidence in different arenas. Selfefficacy goes beyond an evaluation of one’s abilities to include their belief that they can carry out that activity, an important belief that will affect their motivation to pursue various activities. Table 2.1 presents the different areas measured by the Multidimensional Scales of Perceived Self-Efficacy scale (MSPSE; Bandura, 1989).
Self-Efficacy Domain | Sample Item “How well can you…” |
---|---|
Academic Achievement | …learn algebra/reading and writing language skills? |
Self-Regulated Learning | …plan your school work? |
Social Self-Efficacy | …make and keep friends of the opposite sex? |
Resisting Peer Pressure | …resist peer pressure to do things in school that can get you into trouble? |
Enlisting Social Resources | …get teachers/another student/etc. to help you when you get stuck on schoolwork? |
Assertive | …stand up for yourself when you feel you are being treated unfairly? |
Meeting Other’s Expectations | …live up to what your parents/teachers/peers/yourself expect of you? |
Enlisting Parental and Community Support | …get your parent(s)/brothers and sisters/etc. to help you with a problem? |
Leisure-Time Skill and Extracurricular Activities | …learn sports/dance/music skills? |
Note: Response items: 1 = Not Well at All, 3= Not Too Well, 5 = Pretty Well, and 7 = Very Well
In an analysis of all the CTYI students who completed the MSPSE (N = 936), patterns of self-efficacy were identified. Nearly three quarters of CTYI students (n = 681; 72.1%; the Confident Majority and Superstars classes) were confident in all domains, with one in four being extremely confident. Highly confident in their academic and social skills, ability to be assertive and to meet others’ expectations, confidence among the Confident Majority only dipped slightly in their ability to manage their learning, garner social support when needed, and be successful in extracurricular or leisure activities. The Superstars class was confident in all these domains. The two most confident classes were made up almost exclusively of students with Resilient or High Resilient personality types, suggesting their flexibility in adapting to variable situations is associated with their confidence they will be successful in diverse activities.
One study of self-efficacy evaluated the 2015 students (N = 477) by their confidence in specific subject areas: general mathematics, algebra, biology, reading/writing, foreign language, and social studies (O’Reilly et al., 2018). While the largest subset of students had high self-efficacy in all subject areas (46% of the sample in that study), one subset (35%) had high confidence in their mathematics abilities, but low confidence in the other humanities-related subjects. CTYI students in the smallest subset (19%) lacked confidence in math, but were quite confident in science and the humanities.
Positive Perfectionism
Perfectionism, “the tendency to demand of others or of oneself an extremely high or even flawless level of performance” (APA, 2020, Perfectionism), among students with gifts and talents has been the focus of a great deal of research attention since the early 1990’s. One of the most widely accepted models of perfectionism describes three types: self-oriented (having unrealistically high expectations of themselves); socially prescribed (perceiving others have unrealistically high expectations of them); and other-oriented (having unrealistically high expectations for others). Much recent research in perfectionism has explored these three types.
Striving for perfection can be a healthy approach to demands. In fact, positive strivings have been found to correlate with adaptive outcomes, such as positive mood and emotion (affect), conscientiousness, motivation to master a task, and a sense of personal agency (an internal locus of control). In the three-dimension model of perfectionism, positive strivings may be measured by self-oriented perfectionism. Among the CTYI and CAT students from the 2015 and 2016 studies, self-oriented perfectionism was high and did not differ by program. Females in both programs, however, had higher selforiented perfectionism than males. The personality profiles differed significantly in their perfectionism scores. The High Resilients, the most adaptive and confident students, and the Overcontrollers, who tended to be less emotionally stable, had the highest selforiented perfectionism scores. These two groups had very different scores in Socially Prescribed Perfectionism, however, which is likely to result in different outcomes.
A Minority Needing Supports
The majority of CTYI students in these studies had a positive psychological profile. Two-thirds of them were confident and had desirable personality characteristics. A quarter to a third, however, may need support to bolster their self-beliefs and to flourish in their environments. Self-concept and self-efficacy develop from experiences. As a child engages with their environment, they have success or do not, they receive feedback from others: praise or criticism, encouragement or discouragement. Through these experiences, they develop beliefs about who they are and what they like and can do. Personality develops from an inborn temperament, shaped by the response of the child’s environment. A newborn with fearful tendencies may experience a caring environment in which they can thrive or a less nurturant one in which their fearfulness is exacerbated. How adults and peers respond to the developing child will affect their beliefs about themselves, and how others respond is affected by the child’s temperament.
Personality Challenges
The CTYI secondary students in the 2015 sample who were classified as Overcontrollers (24.2%) or Undercontrollers (9.6%) may experience challenges not faced by their resilient peers. Overcontrollers, a majority of whom – but not all – were female (63.8%), may be at particular risk for internalizing symptoms, such as depression and anxiety. They were highly introverted, meaning they prefer to avoid overstimulation, such as crowds and high-noise settings. Overcontrollers also tend to score high in neuroticism – they report being more likely to feeling depressed or to worry excessively. A supportive environment for Overcontrollers would recognize these differences, offering quiet spaces and activities with small groups or pairs. Professional counselors or attentive caregivers may help them reframe stressful events to reduce their anxiety.
Undercontrollers have been found to exhibit more externalizing problems, such as impulsivity, interpersonal conflict, and aggression . Their high extraversion means they would likely seek out peers to interact with, but their high disagreeableness may make it more difficult to develop friendships. These students may benefit from social skills training and positive feedback when they behave in a friendly manner. Their impulsivity can make peers uncomfortable. Teaching self-regulation directly and rewarding such behaviors may support Undercontrollers in developing friendships. Undercontrollers at CTYI were highly conscientiousness, which suggests they will have protection from some of the difficulties found among undercontrollers in the general population.
Self-Belief Challenges
Poor self-concept was found in 19.3% of CTYI students in the 2012 study, which included primary students, who tend to have positive self-concepts. Some CTYI students in the study had high self-concepts in all areas except their physical abilities. Self-concept can be boosted with affirmation, but having actual opportunities to succeed is more effective in changing self-beliefs. Challenge is important for these capable students, but succeeding at incrementally more difficult tasks at a lower level – working up to the ultimate challenge – will be better for self-concept development than repeatedly failing by attempting the ultimate challenge to start. In all cases, adults should be attending to what the child needs.
Academic self-efficacy was high among the CTYI secondary students and three of the classes with high self-efficacy included the majority of students: the Confident Majority (high self-efficacy across the board), the Confident Pushovers (high in all areas except ability to resist peer pressure), and the Superstars (very high in all areas). Three smaller classes, however, had less positive profiles. The Pushovers, the Insecure, and the Need a Boost classes made up 22.3% of the students in the 2013-2015 studies (N = 936). The Pushovers (n = 25) had relatively low scores overall, but were notably least likely to say they could resist peer pressure to engage in troubling behavior. The small group of Insecure students (n = 18) did not have concerns about peer pressure, but they had low self-efficacy in all other areas, with the exception of academics. The Need a Boost class (n = 165) lacked confidence in their learning skills, their ability to get help from others, and to do extracurriculars. In other areas, they had more confidence, but it was modest. An emphasis on how to get support from others would be beneficial to CTYI students in all three low-confidence classes: recognizing resources, learning how to ask for help when they need it, general social skills training.
Perfectionism Challenges
While self-oriented perfectionism has been found to be associated with positive outcomes, socially prescribed perfectionism has been linked to negative outcomes, including maladaptive motivational goals, negative affect, neuroticism, distress, eating disorders, and anxiety. The perception that others are expecting you to be perfect can be debilitating. Students in the Overcontroller class, who were high in positive expectations for their own perfection, were also high in their belief that others expect them to be perfect. These students, primarily female but including several males, are likely to be in need of psychological support.
Adult behavior, particularly that of parents, has been implicated in the development of perfectionistic beliefs, both positive and negative. Children may learn to strive for perfection or to be concerned about being evaluated negatively by observing the model of significant others or through being rewarded for such striving or punished for not doing so. They also learn through their own experience of striving for excellence, by thinking about what has occurred. Parents have an important role in their child’s development of these concerns. Their responsiveness to the child’s needs is critical to developing positive attitudes about their efforts to achieve. Research has supported the most positive outcomes for children raised with a balance between parents’ demandingness and responsiveness. An excess of demandingness in parenting may contribute to an unhealthy concern that others are evaluating you. Responsive parents are willing to give in to their child at times, realizing that their child needs to have confidence in their own ability to make choices and affect their own lives. Such a sense of agency will not develop if parents are constantly demanding. Table 2.2 describes the path parents set for their child through their modeling, responsiveness, and demandingness. It is important to note that all contributing factors highlighted in Table 2.2 are based on the child’s perceptions. An outsider may see a behavior as demanding or a model as positive or negative, but the child’s own perceptions of the behavior or model are what matter.
Outcome: Positive Striving | Outcome: Negative Concerns |
---|---|
Parent expectations for high standards (demandingness) | Parent expectations for high standards (demandingness) |
Parent models striving with positive attitudes toward failure / mistakes as part of learning | Parent models concerns with negative/ fearful attitudes toward failure / mistakes |
Parent encourages high achievement via warm, positive messaging (responsiveness) | Parent demands high achievement via harsh, critical teaching (demandingness) |
Parent is accepting of child’s efforts | Parent is rejecting of child’s efforts |
Adapted from Fletcher & Speirs Neumeister, 2017
Many people believe that human characteristics, such as intelligence and personality, are fixed within a person, but substantial evidence indicates that, while generally stable, one’s intelligence is affected by resources (e.g., exposure to books, access to technology, experience with excellent teachers, etc.) and that many aspects of one’s personality can change in response to the environment. Dweck (2006) found that students who held the belief that intelligence was fixed (e.g., “I am smart.”) were less likely to persist in the face of a difficult task, while those who believed it could be changed with opportunities for learning and practice were more likely to continue trying. She called these beliefs a growth mindset. Many schools have implemented programs to teach students about the significance of effort in achievement, attempting to overcome students’ fixed mindsets.
Among CTYI and CAT students, average scores were below 3.5, the point where beliefs would be considered “fixed”. CTYI females had more fixed beliefs than the males in both CTYI and CAT. All students had slightly stronger beliefs in the fixedness of personality. Implicit beliefs were not different among the personality classes, but both the Pushovers and Confident Pushovers had the most fixed beliefs about intelligence. Learning about how both intelligence and personality can change may help these students who claimed to be unable to resist peer pressure.
Chapter 3
The ability to have positive, lasting significant relationships is a critical human need. People of all ages are motivated by this need4 . They will avoid activities that come between them and people with whom they have (or wish to have) a connection and they will pursue activities that foster relationships.
Being excluded from peers contributes to increased aggression, anxiety, and depression. Even expecting to be rejected by peers can lead to social anxiety and withdrawal. One study found the experience of pain associated with social rejection is similar to that of physical pain. Students in the “brain” crowd of one study had an increase in internalizing distress as they transitioned from childhood to adolescence, suggesting these students faced uniquely difficult stressors.
CTYI students have intellectual abilities different from their peers, as evidenced by their exceptional scores on standardized tests. They may not have intellectual peers in the same classroom or even the same school, creating challenges to friendship formation. Studies of popularity have found that high achievers in primary grades are often popular, but in secondary classes high achievers are less likely to be considered popular. Significant research, cited in Volume 2, Chapter 3, indicates potential challenges to friendship for CTYI students. Considering the importance of friendship to thriving in school, an important component of our studies with CTYI students focused on their relationships with peers.
Most CTYI students believed they had positive relations with peers, especially those with positive self-concepts in physical appearance and abilities. They had confidence in their ability to make friends “Pretty Well,” to stand up for themselves among peers and to resist pressure to engage in unacceptable behavior. About 20% of students did not believe they could stand up for themselves or resist peer pressure, however. On average, CTYI secondary students believed they could get help from peers to solve a social problem, but they were less confident they could get help from a peer when they were stuck on schoolwork. Students in the Overcontroller personality class were less likely than others to believe they could get help from peers on social problems and also were not confident they could live up to peers’ expectations (See Figure 3.1).
A majority of CTYI students did not consider themselves to be ostracized; they did not believe they were ignored or excluded by peers. Many of the Overcontroller students who were highly introverted, did believe they were at least sometimes ostracized by peers. Students in the low self-efficacy classes – the Pushovers, the Need a Boost, and especially the Insecure – were more likely to perceive they were excluded than their more confident CTYI peers.
In the early 1980’s, Larry Coleman and Tracy Cross spoke with hundreds of gifted students participating in the Tennessee Governor’s Schools about their social experiences. Coleman had previously proposed that giftedness is stigmatizing, and gifted students know that if others become aware of their exceptional abilities, they will be unable to have normal social interactions. From conversations with the students, Coleman and Cross (1988) learned about the conditions that led students to manage information about themselves so they could have normal social interactions and how they went about it. Gifted students might allow their abilities to be highly visible; or they would disidentify from their giftedness, behaving in ways counter to how they perceive a gifted person would, such as rebelling; or they would attempt to make themselves or their giftedness invisible. One thing that came up repeatedly in their interviews was students’ belief that they were different from peers. To further study this phenomenon, Coleman and Cross created a questionnaire that was modified for research with CTYI students. In the first part, questions asked how they believed others see them (the same as other students or different from other students) and how they considered themselves different from peers. Once again, the personality classes responded differently.
Overcontrollers and Undercontrollers were more likely to believe they were seen as different. Overcontrollers agreed most strongly that they get bored more quickly with small talk than others. Although the High Resilient students considered themselves most serious about learning, they did not believe peers get in the way of their learning. Undercontrollers, who were least agreeable, were also most likely to say other students get in the way of their learning. All CTYI students agreed that they prefer to work independently. CAT students had similar scores, except when it comes to viewing other students as getting in the way. They did not generally agree.
The second part of Cross and Coleman’s questionnaire was based on their findings that the conditions under which the stigma of giftedness had its effects differed. Some situations were more threatening to being “outed” as a gifted student than others. They tested this finding quantitatively with a series of scenarios, carefully crafted to elicit a response to these varying threats. The least threatening situation was to publicly show they know a discrete fact that other students did not. The scenario they created was of students complaining about not knowing the meaning of the word onomatopoeia. Asked how they would respond if they knew the meaning, students could choose options along a continuum of telling the truth to lying. The response options were developed from information given in student interviews. Students may deflect attention from their true beliefs (truth) by placating (agreeing with some aspect of the comment, before exposing true feelings), copping out (changing the subject), or covering up by using words that are related to the conversation, but do not reveal anything about the person’s self, or by giving a false response (lying). Another threatening scenario described a situation when others were not interested in learning, but the gifted student wanted to learn. For this situation, a scenario describes a substitute teacher being taunted by peers. The most threatening exposure is in the Biology Exam scenario, where others are complaining about the difficulty of a test the gifted student found easy. Cross et al. (1991) found many students responded to the Onomatopoeia scenario by saying they would tell the truth. The majority of students indicated they would placate in response to the Substitute Teacher scenario. The Biology Exam elicited the broadest range of responses, with some students comfortable telling the truth, but more being likely to cop out or even lie. Scenarios from the 2015 and 2016 surveys are below.
Scenario #1
Setting: In the cafeteria line, several people from your class are discussing the life science exam.
Taisce: Man! Wasn’t that test impossible? I must have spent 10 minutes trying to think of examples of the major biomes.
Corey: I blew the whole thing, even though I studied really hard.
Devin: I probably failed it too.
Devin says to Shannon, “I bet you breezed through it and didn’t even open the book to study.” Actually, Shannon spent several hours studying and thought it wasn’t a difficult test.
If you were Shannon, what would you be MOST inclined to say?
Please circle your choice.
A (Preface No Answer) | B (Lie) | C (Placate) | D (Truth) | E (Cop-Out) |
---|---|---|---|---|
“Tests can be hard sometimes.” | “Yeah, that exam was a pain.” | “I probably studied as hard as you did, but the test wasn’t too hard.” | “I thought it was kind of easy.” | “How long did you study?” |
Scenario #2
Setting: A group of students is discussing a class lecture as they leave the classroom.
Brady: I think it’s crazy that Mr. O’Reilly expects us to remember all of that material in Chapter 10 for the test in Literature!
Kieran: What does he think – that we have nothing better to do than memorize that stuff from the book?
Quinn: Some of those words are hard. I don’t even understand what he means by “onomatopoeia,” do you guys?
They all shake their heads, with the exception of Jamie (who has said nothing to this point). They turn to Jamie. Quinn says, “How about you, Jamie? Knowing you, you probably know it. Right?”
Jamie understands all of the terms and knows that onomatopoeia is nothing more than a word that describes a sound.
If you were Jamie, which would you be MOST inclined to say?
Please circle your choice.
A (Truth) | B (Placate) | C (Cop-Out) | D (Preface No Answer) | E (Lie) |
---|---|---|---|---|
“It means a word that imitates a sound, like ‘crash’ or ‘bang.’” | It’s hard to remember those words, but I think it means a word that describes a sound, like ‘crash’ or ‘bang.’” | “I think you’re right, Mr. O’Reilly is expecting too much.” | “It’s not easy to remember those terms, no one can keep them straight.” | “I have no idea what those words mean, either.” |
Scenario #3
Setting: In the hallway, between classes:
Pat: Wasn’t that substitute teacher for Mrs. Flannery awful? I couldn’t figure out what she was trying to say about the Western Expansion. She really lost me.
Reagan: How about what Pete pulled on her, pretending he was sick and ready to throw up on her desk?
Aidan: She even believed it. I wish I had thought of that one! I would rather have spent the period in the clinic instead of sitting in that class. Everyone but Kelly nodded their heads in agreement.
Reagan looked at Kelly and asked, “Didn’t you think that was hysterical?” Kelly felt that the substitute had started an interesting topic, but Pete had made it impossible for her to teach. Kelly thought Pete had been unnecessarily rude.
If you were Kelly, which would you be MOST inclined to say?
Please circle your choice.
A (Cop-Out) | B (Placate) | C (Truth) | D (Preface No Answer) | E (Lie) |
---|---|---|---|---|
“I wonder when Mrs. Flannery is coming back.” | “Some of it was funny, but Pete shouldn’t have gone that far.” | “I thought the class got out of control, Pete went too far.” | “Pete can be funny sometimes.” | “Pete was funny. The substitute was asking for it.” |
CTYI and CAT students responded similarly to the scenarios (see Figure 3.3). As in the 1980’s study, there were more truth-oriented responses in the Onomatopoeia scenario, more placating in the Substitute Teacher scenario, and a variety of responses in the Biology Exam scenario. Notably, 26% of the Irish students chose the “lie” option in the Biology Exam scenario, versus 12% of US students in Cross et al.’s (1991) study. The implication is that there is a high social cost to have one’s giftedness exposed to peers among CTYI and CAT students. Interestingly, the more students believed they were seen as different from peers and were less like them, the less likely they were to respond evasively to scenarios (i.e., tell the truth). This was most true in the Substitute Teacher scenario and CAT students had stronger correlations than CTYI students. As they more strongly agreed that they preferred to work independently and were more serious about learning than their peers, the more likely they were to choose more truthful options about Petey disrupting their learning. Conversely, as they preferred to work with peers or did not agree they were more serious than peers, they were more likely to hide their true feelings from peers and chose less truthful options.
To help us better understand the social experience of gifted students, the cross-cultural study of 2013 explored this topic. In the 90 interviews conducted across elementary, middle, and high school aged5 , gifted students in five countries (Ireland, United States, United Kingdom, France, and Korea), the social experiences described fell into six themes:
Positive Competition was only seen among UK and South Korean students, but the other themes described common experiences of all the students. Table 3.1 gives examples of CTYI students’ comments in each social experience theme.
-
Awareness of Others’ Expectations
-
Pressure
-
Concerned About Peers’ Feelings
-
Comfortable Among Gifted Peers
-
Confused by Response of Peers
-
Positive Competition
In the study, Irish primary school students were classified as elementary, Irish secondary school students at junior cycle as middle school and Irish secondary students at senior cycle as high school.
Theme | Participant ID | Comment |
---|---|---|
Awareness of Others’ Expectations | IRMM2 | Sometimes after football training my Dad would ask me a maths question and I might get it wrong because I’m tired. And he would be surprised about this and also in school my teacher would be very surprised if I get anything wrong which puts extra pressure on me and raises expectations. |
IRMF1 | The teacher was disappointed in me which made me a bit annoyed and sad. | |
Pressure | IRHF1 | It’s a struggle with school where girls in my class will just comment on it. If they get above me in a test, it’s a big thing for them and they really, they don’t let it go. Constantly there’s pressure there to do well just so you’re not pointed out in class for not doing well. |
IRMF1 | That’s why I don’t think it’s good to be the best, even though I want to be, because everyone expects a lot and when you don’t reach it, people are disappointed. | |
Concerned About Peers’ Feelings | IRMF1 | If they asked me if I found it [an exam] easy, I’d say it wasn’t that hard. I’d say I tried and I hope I do well but I wouldn’t straight out say it was so easy and I can’t believe you found it so hard because that’s just mean. Q: Why is it mean if it’s the truth? A: Even though you finding it easy made you feel good about yourself, if you put someone down for finding it hard. Finding it hard was stressful enough anyway so you’re just adding to the badness. Q: You’re worried about hurting people’s feelings? A: I think it’s because I was bullied for my intellectual abilities so I don’t want to be mean to people because of theirs. |
Comfortable Among Gifted Peers | IRHF1 | I was really shocked. It was strange. My first class in Novel Writing we were discussing Ulysses and what was wrong with Twilight and it was crazy. Everyone had very similar interests to me and I fitted in very quickly. |
Confused by Response of Peers | IRMM3 | Sometimes they make a bit of fun of me because I always know the answer. It’s not just me though, as they make fun of people who don’t know any answers. It doesn’t make sense really. |
IRHF2 | I have a few friends who say that “2 weeks after DCU, you can talk about it but after that if you mention it I won’t talk to you”. I find that quite offensive because they have friends outside of school and they talk about them and I don’t give out about that because people have other friends but they don’t want to talk about CTYI because they don’t want me to and I think it’s a bit much. |
Note: Participant ID is country code (IR=Ireland), age group (E = elementary, M = middle, H = high), sex (F = Female, M = Male), and subject reference number (1–3).
The stigma of giftedness was evident in all the countries of this study. Irish students’ comments can be seen in Table 3.2. Students clearly wanted to have normal interactions, but were inhibited in some ways connected to their high abilities. CTYI students were keenly aware of their visibility as highly able and many reported being rejected by peers. Bragging, being “boasty,” was viewed quite negatively by many of the students in the study. Concern for peers’ feelings was often given as a reason for not drawing attention to one’s performance.
Subtheme | Participant ID | Comment |
---|---|---|
Awareness of Visibility | IRMF3 | I’m proud of being a nerd. Overall it is a positive experience |
IRHM1 | Your reputation precedes you. When you get introduced to things and they’d say this person did X and Y and you’re seen as that rather than who you are. You don’t want that to be seen as what defines you. You want to be seen as who you are | |
Rejection by Peers | IREF2 | My friend asks me for an answer and I tell her that I can’t tell her because it’s a test, sometimes, she like, doesn’t play with me anymore |
IRHM3 | Sometimes if I’m trying to be friends with someone and I’m smart, they might reject me a bit. They’re more interested in being friends with someone who’s good at sports or music. | |
Awareness of Jealousy | IREM2 | I don’t talk about it [my abilities], just like, in case there’s people who might be jealous, so I just keep it to myself. |
IRMM3 | Some of my friends are not that happy about how well I do in tests. I wouldn’t mind, it’s mostly the ones who are smart themselves. They can get obsessed with doing better than me. | |
Few Close Friends | IRHF1 | They just have me around for a laugh over a random fact. I don’t have any close friends I could talk to. I’m almost comedic to them. They find me a bit of a laugh. |
IREM1 | At school, I don’t have many friends and that’s probably because of my ability. | |
Avoid Bragging | IRHM3 | I don’t like to flaunt my results and make people feel bad. |
IRHF3 | I think I’d feel like I was bragging because others found it difficult and I wouldn’t want them to feel bad because they clearly worked hard. |
Note: Participant ID is country code (IR=Ireland), age group (E = elementary, M = middle, H = high), sex (F = Female, M = Male), and subject reference number (1–3).
The ways in which CTYI students cope with the stigmatizing effects of giftedness were consistent with those of students in the other countries. They hid their talents, conformed to others’ behavior, helped peers when they could, and focused on themselves without regard for what others were expecting from them. Table 3.3 includes examples of CTYI students’ coping strategies.
Theme | Participant ID | Comment |
---|---|---|
Hiding | IRHF1 | My English teacher, because I’m good at essays, keeps pointing it out to the class and I’ve started not completing homework assignments because she always reads out mine. |
IRMM3 | I’m trying to deflect attention away from myself. I can gauge their answer and fit mine in to what they tell me….It’s easier not to draw attention to yourself. | |
Conformity | IREF3 | I don’t really think that I’m special and all. I just try and fit in. |
IREM2 | Well, I…I just try and act like I’m just like everyone else. | |
Helping | IRMM1 | I help people with stuff. They ask a lot of the time. If they’re stuck on homework they might ask me. |
IRMF1 | They slag me but I think they appreciate that I’m smart because I can help for tests and stuff and in class I can help them as well. | |
Self-focus | IRHF2 | I’d rather feel under pressure from myself than other people because when it’s from others, you can’t fix it. |
IRHM2 | You shouldn’t let other people’s opinions of how smart or enthusiastic you are affect how much you contribute. | |
IRMF3 | I’m really happy with myself. I take pride in my work. I’m not ashamed of doing well because of what people might think. Other people’s opinions wouldn’t stop me from doing well because there will always be people like me. |
Note: Participant ID is country code (IR=Ireland), age group (E = elementary, M = middle, H = high), sex (F = Female, M = Male), and subject reference number (1–3).
We did find an age-related pattern among the gifted students participating in the study (see Figure 3.4). Elementary-aged students were proud of the recognition their outstanding abilities received. They were happy when their parents or teachers were proud of their achievements. By middle school, students began to express an awareness of problems associated with their abilities. They were subjected to higher expectations than peers from parents, teachers, and even classmates. They experienced peers’ jealousy, rejection, and demands for help. By high-school age, these high-ability students had learned to accept these difficulties and developed coping strategies for dealing with them. Importantly, the high school students in the cross-cultural study were participating in gifted programs at the time of the research. It is likely that some students who learn in middle school about the challenges of higher expectations and peer rejection or demandingness will decide to leave such programs. Better understanding these difficulties and creating more positive environments will help more students achieve to their potential.
The information gathered in the cross-cultural study made it possible to create a questionnaire that explored social experiences in greater depth, with a larger number of students. In 2018, CTYI students completed the Social Experiences of Gifted Students Scale (SEGSS), which asked the frequency and emotional response to 53 experiences. From the 53 items of the SEGSS, 7 factors were identified;
-
Top of the Class: Things that happen when smart; I perform better and other students know it.
-
Helping Expectations: My abilities lead others to expect sharing/helping; associated with their hurt feelings/envy.
-
More Serious: I don’t get them because I’m more serious.
-
Pressure to Achieve: Pressure from others to always do well and be right.
-
Peer Rejection: I was rejected or made fun of.
-
Adult Expectations: Teachers and parents expect me to excel.
-
Hiding: Hiding behaviors so as not to be seen as different.
The most frequent experiences were related to being visible for their abilities, Top of the Class. These experiences were associated with the most positive feelings (Figure 3.10). CTYI students frequently experienced pressure from others to always do well and always be right (Pressure to Achieve) and this pressure was the experience that felt worst to the female CTYI students. Adult Expectations from parents and teachers to do well in school were frequent, but did not feel bad. Expectations that they would help peers occurred once in a while. For males, especially, this was associated with particularly good feelings. CTYI students had experienced the need to hide their abilities – less often for males than females and nonconforming students. It was not such a negative feeling for males. More Serious items were associated with confusion about other students’ behavior – it made no sense that others wanted to get out of schoolwork or copy theirs. This happened sometimes, but not often. It felt mostly good when it did occur. Fortunately, CTYI students reported that Peer Rejection occurred infrequently, but this was accompanied by bad feelings.
Students were asked to “Please share below any comments about the experiences listed above or any other social experiences related to your high academic abilities.” Through these comments (see Table 3.4), we see clear support for the experiences identified in the questionnaire. It is heartening that many students have positive social experiences in school (see the “All Good” section in Table 3.4), which can challenge the stereotype of the isolated, rejected “nerd”. We know that a majority of students at CTYI are not likely to be socially awkward, based on the analysis of personality and self-efficacy in Chapter 2, but the experiences and emotions of those who do not have a resilient personality or high self-efficacy are important to understand.
Year in School | Sex | Comment |
---|---|---|
Top of Class | ||
3 | Female | I stand out. People, particularly my peers, expect me to never be wrong. It is a lot of pressure and can make me stressed or depressed. |
3 | Male | In some groups it’s an annoyance to have to use simpler vocabulary and act |
2 | Female | I am often criticised for enjoying learning |
2 | Female | Everyone hated me in school because I read books |
5 | Male | I wouldn’t try answering a question I was not certain of the answer as if I’m wrong I often feel like the class gets excited at the act I was wrong. Makes me feel awkward embarrassed and that this is unfair. I should feel comfortable suggesting an answer I was unsure of. |
6 | Female | I don’t like feeling pressured or feeling like I’m under a microscope. Sometimes people do better in tests and they shove it in my face because I’m the smart kid. I can’t get 100% all the time despite what people may think. But I love helping people and I don’t like to share any results so no one feels bad. Sometimes I study for half an hour and I get a B and they study for days and get a B. It’s not what either of us are worth, it’s just a B |
5 | Female | There were no classes for more advanced students. I am always forced into a class with people who are at a slower pace than me |
2 | Female | I used to act like I didn’t study because people would laugh at me for it. |
Pressure to Achieve | ||
1 | Female | When I get a lower result than what my friends expected me to get, I felt really disappointed. Sometimes my friends would playfully say that like “you’ll be fine you have probably been up late all night studying!” I know they are only playing but sometimes it’s just a little frustrating |
2 | Female | I do have quite a few friends but I do feel sometimes that people don’t like me and I often feel pressured to do well in school |
5 | Female | Felt pressure to constantly succeed or like there was an expectation to always be the best academically but never got much praise or encouragement |
3 | Male | People put too much pressure on me to get things right |
4 | Female | I feel under a lot of pressure about exams and tests and I sometimes think it makes me do worse |
3 | Female | My teachers push me a lot, resulting in me sometimes breaking down |
3 | Female | I stand out. People, particularly my peers, expect me to never be wrong. It is a lot of pressure and can make me stressed or depressed. |
1 | Male | Other students often expect that I get everything correct/ high school/grade. I enjoy CTYI because it allows me to meet a lot of great minds and intellectuals. |
2 | Female | Teachers & parents often put unnecessary pressure on us |
3 | Female | Everyone in school knows I’m smart but don’t really care other than always expecting me to get everything right. It’s annoying being in a school where everybody is fairly stupid. They don’t care about their education and get in the way of my learning new topics. |
5 | Male | It stresses me out that I care more about doing well on tests & studying than my friends do. It really stresses me out when I perform below the high expectations that I have of myself. |
5 | Female | People like me for me, mostly not my brain, though I felt great pressure to perform (esp. in JC) |
3 | Female | I don’t like girls in my school constantly needing to know my grades because I feel bad if they don’t do as well as me. Girls always expect me to get so high and roll their eyes at me if I meet their expectations. I’m scared people will think of me differently if I tell them my grades. |
4 | Female | I don’t have extremely high academic ability. I would not care about this so much, only everyone seems to overestimate my abilities. When my parents, teachers or peers mention how “smart” I am, I feel as though I’m letting them down. This has begun to bleed into other aspects of my life, and now I often find that I am critical of my abilities to do even the simplest of tasks. |
3 | I was better than the majority of my class but there were two better than me and I was quite worried that I would measure up. I was also under pressure from my family to socialise more. | |
3 | Female | Classmates expect a lot from me academically. |
3 | Female | People make fun of me for using big words. I hate it. People expect me to do amazingly but I can’t. People always ask to copy my work and I don’t know how to say no. |
1 | Female | I get annoyed when people expect me to be perfect at everything |
4 | Female | A lot of things only happened in primary school. But I think that is because my secondary school has an academic scholarship programme so people in my school don’t really care as much about if someone is gifted. They admire people’s talents rather than get jealous and be hurtful. In relation to other people’s academic talents I rarely measure others up to myself and how they do academically has no bearing on my feelings. I do however measure myself up to others and am often upset when I don’t do as well as I think I should |
4 | Female | I’m just a private and quiet person. I don’t talk much to my classmates unless they’re good friends (about my achievements). I don’t know if they’re jealous. My biggest issue is that they sometimes view my level as something good to pass or overtake, like if I don’t bother and I get a C, people are surprised and say, “Oh wow I did better.” |
2 | Female | My friends often expect/predict that I would get high scores on tests. |
Adult Expectations | ||
2 | Female | When people (parents or teachers) expected me to do well it made me work harder and I felt happy that they had expectations of me |
5 | Male | I enjoy helping. I never really care what people think. People have high expectations that I fail to meet often. |
4 | Female | I go to a quite academic school, and while others are praised for doing well, for me it is expected. |
3 | Female | People just expect you to do well sometimes idk |
Helping Expectations | ||
3 | Female | I like to feel challenged academically, I enjoy people having high standards of me and don’t mind showing my academic ability as it is something I am proud of and embrace. I am happy to help classmates who struggle and do not mind them putting high expectations on me as it is a challenge I strive to meet. I never try to hide or feel embarrassed of my academic ability |
2 | Female | People copy my work, but I know it’s because I will probably get the answers right. People assume that I will get high grades etc. and that makes me feel stressed. |
2 | Female | Some people in school hardly speak to me except to ask me for answers |
3 | Female | Sometimes I’d be saying something and my friends would stop me and go “Ok. I don’t understand.” or “English, please.” I feel like I am known for being smart. “Of course you didn’t find it hard”, “Let me look at your homework/ Can you help me do this?/ What do I do?”`` |
2 | Female | I suffer from Asperger’s so I’ve always found it hard to make friends and understand people’s feelings. I have regularly been asked to let someone copy my homework. I feel different from groups at school but in CTYI everyone is friendly and understandable |
1 | Female | I hate when people ask me what I get in tests, they look at me weirdly. I don’t tell them because I don’t want them to feel bad. I also hate when people ask to copy my homework. |
3 | Female | Many times people in my class have tried to copy my work. I never give them the answers. I try to help them through the work while allowing them to do it themselves. I then feel good because I have helped someone understand something |
4 | Female | I used to feel like the only reason people would come up and talk to me was because they wanted to copy my homework or ask me how to do something related to school work. I didn’t think that they really liked me, or wanted to talk to me. It’s alright now though. |
Hiding | ||
3 | Female | Some people just won’t like you if you’re smart. It makes it easy to be self-deprecating to fit in. |
3 | Male | In some groups it’s an annoyance to have to use simpler vocabulary and act |
1 | Female | I changed my answers to the wrong ones in a Drumcondra test to fit in with my friends (a long time ago) |
2 | Female | I used to act like I didn’t study because people would laugh at me for it. |
2 | Female | I try not to do too well in school so I don’t offend/annoy people. I am not the only smart person in my class. |
3 | Female | I don’t like girls in my school constantly needing to know my grades because I feel bad if they don’t do as well as me. Girls always expect me to get so high and roll their eyes at me if I meet their expectations. I’m scared people will think of me differently if I tell them my grades |
2 | Female | I tried to hide my academic abilities so others would not treat me differently. I moved schools in 4th class but before I moved the teacher and students always expected me to study all the time and love homework so I always got extra homework. When I moved school I tried hiding my abilities so I could be like everyone else |
3 | Male | Often I don’t want to brag if I get good grades because I’m afraid the friends I do have will get annoyed so I often keep quiet. I feel like I’m holding myself back and I hate being isolated when I didn’t want to be. That’s why I love CTYI because there are so many likeminded people. I can openly be myself and have good conversations with everyone. I don’t usually feel pressure from my parents or teachers but I often feel because I usually get good grades that my standards have raised so much I feel I’m disappointing people if I don’t do well. People could often say I thought you were smart and it hurts my feelings. |
2 | Female | I tend to avoid competition so as to not being comparing myself to others. As well as this, I simultaneously try to hide myself and show myself off (in school, academically) so as to receive more challenge, but privately, with us ~competition~ from others (e.g., spelling bee) is not a “fun” challenge. |
2 | Female | I feel like a ‘misfit’ at school and I have a few friends, and when I talk to them, I have to ‘dumb down.’ Whenever I get good scores I try to hide them, but the other students find out anyway. I am envied for this but I feel horrible as I am treated very differently, like an outsider |
3 | Female | I basically suppressed my abilities from 3rd class until 2nd year |
More Serious | ||
3 | Female | I feel like my experiences were very different to a good few of the questions as my grades aren’t the best, I’m just mature and a lot of the time I get annoyed at all the stupid drama school friends were creating but that’s the extent of it. |
4 | Male | I have never cared about anyone’s feelings or opinions of me regarding my ability academically or physically because those people lack any trait I value and are essentially useless |
1 | Male | Being so smart leads to power, and with power comes greed. Many of my classmates were jealous of my academic abilities and used to bully me quite a bit. Eventually they were used to it and didn’t mind it. |
3 | Female | Everyone in school knows I’m smart but don’t really care other than always expecting me to get everything right. It’s annoying being in a school where everybody is fairly stupid. They don’t care about their education and get in the way of my learning new topics. |
4 | Male | I’ve never been good at socializing but I’m getting better. I generally don’t care what others think about me, they all have relationships and go to discos, but I don’t do that stuff. |
5 | Female | Although I have had experiences with jealous students since primary school, as a whole, I have had good experiences with the vast majority of students. I have struggled with jealous students all my life. I’d be much harder on myself and my grades than any of my teachers or my parents |
2 | Female | People copy homework A LOT. And they (immediately after we get tests results back) ask me what I got, and assume that I spend my life studying, when really I just retain information rather easily, and actually enjoy school/homework/learning. If I get an A, they judge. If I get a B, they also judge. It’s like they can’t handle me doing good nor bad. School is sometimes annoying because we have to go at a slow pace sometimes, and it gets boring having to repeat simple concepts. That’s why CTYI is so good - learning is intense and work is challenging (a dream come true compared to real school! :)) |
Peer Rejection | ||
2 | Female | I don’t really like sharing my scores because I don’t think it adds or subtracts from anything, from who I am, and I know others do care and I don’t want to make them feel bad |
6 | Male | Lack of interests in common leads to social isolation |
2 | Female | Some people in school hardly speak to me except to ask me for answers |
3 | Female | Sometimes people treat me differently because I’m intelligent. They don’t accept me in their circle. |
5 | Male | Certain other students become enraged at me, become aggressive over envy of my academic ability |
1 | Female | I changed my answers to the wrong ones in a Drumcondra test to fit in with my friends (a long time ago) |
3 | Male | Being sporty I was able to be more socially in tune than others of my academic ability |
3 | Male | Other people treat you like you are different and make fun of you. |
2 | Female | I do have quite a few friends but I do feel sometimes that people don’t like me and I often feel pressured to do well in school |
2 | Male | Yes; outcast in school, focuses energy on dreams and wishes, most friends are close, few acquaints |
2 | Female | I won a maths competition and everyone was talking about me badly and being jealous, until they heard the prize was only a maths book. |
2 | Female | I used to act like I didn’t study because people would laugh at me for it |
3 | Male | I went to a very small primary school where I was bullied a lot and had no friends but my social skills have been getting better |
6 | Male | School is depressing and lonely. 5th year was a totally exhausting experience. |
2 | Female | I am often criticised for enjoying learning |
3 | Female | Some people just won’t like you if you’re smart. It makes it easy to be self-deprecating to fit in. |
3 | Male | The Centre for Talented Youth, Ireland has relieved a lot of the worries pertaining to the social aspects. It allowed me to connect with other intellectuals of the same age. Ergo, I have a lot more likeminded individuals in my proverbial phonebook. |
2 | Female | I feel like a ‘misfit’ at school and I have a few friends, and when I talk to them, I have to ‘dumb down.’ Whenever I get good scores I try to hide them, but the other students find out anyway. I am envied for this but I feel horrible as I am treated very differently, like an outsider. |
2 | Male | It socially isolates you to be better at something no one cares about |
5 | Male | if you want to fit in you have to be not too smart or too stupid - not in ctyi |
All Good | ||
3 | Female | I like to feel challenged academically, I enjoy people having high standards of me and don’t mind showing my academic ability as it is something I am proud of and embrace. I am happy to help classmates who struggle and do not mind them putting high expectations on me as it is a challenge I strive to meet. I never try to hide or feel embarrassed of my academic ability |
3 | Male | In school I am not ignored for my intelligence nor am I crowded by other students with questions. I am treated like everyone else and am happy with that. |
4 | Female | Like it’s grand. Most things happen that may be good or sad but like so, it’s fine. |
2 | Female | No one cares about grades in my school and no one gets upset about them. |
5 | Female | I usually don’t care what other people think about my abilities and so I don’t know what they think. Most of my friends are just as smart as me |
5 | Female | Why is this all so stereotypical. My social interactions are fairly normal and uninteresting |
4 | Male | So, none of these events taking place alter my mental state in any way. I’ve accepted them as a part of everyday life. It would be asinine to allow so many variables to make me feel any different, as then I would have to want to make others feel and act differently towards me, which is a fruitless endeavor. |
4 | Male | I don’t like other people feeling bad because of me. I like feeling smart. |
2 | Female | When people (parents or teachers) expected me to do well it made me work harder and I felt happy that they had expectations of me. |
2 | Female | School is fun most of the time. |
5 | Male | Nobody really cared, like either about your work or study or you don’t care. I go to a school with very little emphasis on academics. There’s great teachers and high results, but about a third of students do LCA |
5 | Male | I don’t care bro, got ‘em. |
3 | Female | I am fine in school and I have a normal experience there. It is nice to come to CTYI to be around people like me. |
In 2020, Irish students – and most students around the world – had strictly curtailed social experiences with peers. Most students attended online school. For the majority, this was their first experience with virtual schooling. “Schools are,” Tracy Cross says, “first and foremost a social enterprise, where some academic learning goes on” (2018; p. 184). What would that social enterprise be like among CTYI students in online school? Sixteen Irish students (88% female) participated in interviews designed to explore the social experience of online learning during the pandemic. When school was totally online, these students had very little interaction with peers. During school, many peers kept their video off, so students could often not see one another in class. The experience of online classes overall was different, “I think when you’re in person you’ll tend to like ask questions. When you have to go through the trouble of like turning your mic on and like putting your hand up, people just wouldn’t, so the classes were very like silent. Some classes had like a different atmosphere to them” (6th Year Female, #2111). Some students saw only the teacher during the time their classes were online. When asked if classmates were more or less friendly in online school, one student responded, “They were mute in online school” (5th Year Male, #2113).
The effects of social comparison were different in online school. It was not even possible at times to know how another student was doing. In inperson school, body language could be a clue to whether a classmate needed help, but this clue was not accessible when school was online.
“To be honest, you couldn’t tell. I mean, like I said, some people weren’t even on their computers and if their camera is off you really couldn’t tell. And even if their cameras are on everybody has this like mutual blank expression where they just like stare off into the middle distance. That could be going in one ear and out the other or they could be understanding everything, or they could be understanding nothing because the breakout rooms were kind of few and far between a little bit scattered.” (5th Year Female)
In contrast to the frequency of the Top of Class experience in person, it was also not possible for other students to know how well the CTYI students were doing. The stigma of giftedness and the social comparisons that underpin some of the threatening interactions students responded to with coping strategies simply did not happen in online school. Cameras and microphones off and few personal interactions, made for a comparatively sterile social environment.
Some students made opportunities to engage with peers outside of class, through texting, setting up their own online social groups, or joining in others’ groups for activities such as online baking. Remote school may have been a place for introverts to flourish. The CTYI students interviewed were mixed on whether the year of online school during the pandemic was a lonely one. Ten of the sixteen said it was not. They maintained connections to friends (“I was able to talk with my friends. I found the ground.” [5th Year Female]) or stayed active with extracurriculars:
To be honest, I had like loads of things to keep me occupied. Because, like I have a book buying addiction and I need to finish the books that I’ve already bought, and like, I already have like lots of things on my to do list anyway. It was like, oh, learn how to skateboard and learn how to do anatomy or paint. (3rd Year Female, #2115)
Families also kept students from being lonely, “I would not say it was lonely because we have a big house - 6 people. So, no it wouldn’t be lonely. So one of my sisters is real close in age and we get along very well. So definitely no, it wouldn’t be lonely” (5th Year Female, #2102). Other students felt very lonely, even those who had made friends.
One positive outcome of this social isolation was that not a single student saw evidence of bullying while students were attending school remotely. There was no opportunity for this kind of negative interaction while schools dealt with the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. How these unusual experiences may affect the lives of high-ability students in the future remains to be seen.
Relationships with parents and siblings are integral to CTYI students’ well-being. Nearly all students in the studies of self-concept had positive perceptions of their relationship with parents, especially the primary students. On average, they agreed that their parents understand them, they get along well, and that “If I have children of my own, I want to bring them up like my parents raised me.” When asked not just how well they like their parents or feel understood, but how well can they get their support when they need it, most students had high confidence they could get their parents to help with a problem or to take part in school activities. Students in the Overcontroller personality class (high Neuroticism, low Extraversion) consistently were least confident, however, and Resilient students were consistently most confident. Siblings can also be a resource when needed, but students in all personality classes were less confident of their support. Positive attitudes about parent relationships were related to positive attitudes toward school.
An inability to get help from parents may be an indication of overly busy parents or, perhaps, of low responsiveness in an authoritarian or neglecting parenting style. In any case, when a child does not perceive support from parents there may be negative outcomes. Forty-three percent of students in the Overcontroller personality class believed they could not get help from their parents when needed. To support these students, it may be important to provide a stable source of responsiveness. Social connections outside the family can also fulfill belonging needs. Four out of 10 students in the Undercontroller personality class (gregarious, disagreeable) did not think they could get help from siblings when they needed it, while 16% believed they could do so “Very Well.” Although the majority of these Undercontroller students were quite confident their parents would be responsive to their needs, 20% of Undercontrollers did not believe they could get help from parents when needed.
Chapter 4
Gifted students usually enter school ready to learn, but schools are not always prepared to provide what they need (Coleman et al., 2015). Schools tend to be focused on the needs of the average students, who are, by definition, the majority of the student population.
Adams (2021) describes effective programs as having “the following clearly articulated elements: a philosophy, goals, a definition, an identification plan, a coherent curriculum, a scope and sequence, a professional development plan, and an evaluation plan” (p. 128). With appropriate administrative support and resources, such a program can provide the advanced curriculum needed for many gifted students. A new movement in the field of gifted education proposes an emphasis on talent development, a focus on developing talents in a specific domain, rather than creating general programs to serve all students at an advanced level. In their School-Based Conception of Giftedness and Talent Development, T. Cross and Cross (2021) go beyond programs to recommend a whole-school focus on the development of talent among all students, not only those identified as gifted. In this conception, all students receive opportunities to learn at an advanced level. Those who are successful and interested in the subject area are given the supports and resources they need to continue developing in that area, resulting in a motivated student who can learn at their own pace. The nascent stage of gifted education in Ireland suggests this may be a time when adopting a talent development model is possible.
The type of instruction that is most effective for gifted learners is not entirely different from what is effective for all students. What is known to be effective for all students is also known to be effective with gifted students, but there are critical differences. Gifted students need curriculum that is more challenging than that for average students, offered at a pace that matches their rate of learning (Tomlinson, 2005). They need advanced materials, and many prefer more abstract content and tasks than their peers. The ability to pursue topics of interest more broadly and deeply will satisfy their desire to learn in-depth, supporting their motivation in school. Many schools seek to cater for gifted students through curriculum differentiation. When differentiation is the framework for providing gifted education, teachers learn what their students already know through preassessments, then offer instruction at an appropriate level to groups or individual students in the same classroom. Effective curriculum differentiation requires a commitment of time and resources. Without strong support for teacher training, time for planning, and materials or assistance needed to teach a variety of lessons, differentiation will not be successful as a means of serving high-ability students (Hertberg-Davis, 2009).
Another effective method of providing the advanced instruction gifted students need is through acceleration. Grade-skipping is the form of acceleration that is best known, but there are actually many forms. Southern and Jones (2015) describe 20 different types, including grade-skipping, but also subject-matter acceleration, curriculum compacting, and concurrent/ dual enrollment, for example. All of these practices, including grade-skipping, have been found to be very effective. Although grade-skipping is often avoided for fears of causing social and emotional harm to the accelerated student, research evidence supports the opposite: gains for students both academically and in the social/emotional realm (Rogers, 2015).
In a 2014 study of more than 800 Irish teachers, school leaders, and other staff from across the country (J. Cross et al., 2014), 73.4% of respondents reported that grade acceleration was not allowed in their school. Most teachers indicated they were differentiating their instruction for high-ability students, which they described as doing a few times per week through asking higher level questions, offering more challenging tasks and individual projects, and grouping students by ability level. A closer analysis of the teachers’ reported practices (Hinch et al., 2018) focused on only those that were likely to be exclusively beneficial to students with high ability:
-
assigning reading of more advanced level work,
-
eliminating curricular material that students have mastered,
-
and substituting different assignments for students who have mastered regular classroom work.
The number of teachers who reported regularly engaging in all three of these practices with gifted students, but not average students – actual differentiation – was only 3% of the total number of teachers. When teachers do not assign advanced level work, eliminate mastered material, and substitute different assignments when the work has been mastered, their gifted students will be receiving inadequate instruction.
A study of 1,440 parents about their 1,914 children who had attended CTYI (J. Cross, Cross et al., 2019) found a majority reported their children were happy in school (63.2%) and liked it (56.1%), but most parents (72.1%) were dissatisfied with their child’s educational experiences. They did not believe their CTYI-attending children were being challenged in school. They reported more than half of the children (54.5%) were not receiving assignments targeting their ability level. Parents of 71% of the secondary students reported they never received more challenging or complex assignments than their classmates. Although 85% of teachers reported they were differentiating the curriculum for their highability learners (J. Cross et al., 2014), this study of parents suggests that figure was not representative of the experience of high-ability students across Ireland.
In general, CTYI students in these studies were confident in their academic abilities. Primary students, in particular, saw themselves as good at reading and mathematics. All students considered their reading abilities to be strong. In an examination of CTYI students’ self-efficacy for learning in math, science, and humanities (O’Reilly et al., 2018), the majority of students had high selfefficacy in all subject areas (46%), one subset (35%) had high confidence in their mathematics abilities, but low confidence in the other humanities-related subjects. CTYI students in the smallest subset (19%) lacked confidence in math, but were quite confident in science and the humanities. Despite an average high confidence level, not all CTYI students will be confident in all subject areas.
Gifted students “have a right to learn something new every day” (Siegle, 2007). Waiting for others to learn material they already know is a common experience that can lead to boredom, frustration, underachievement, or even dropping out of school altogether. Curricular differentiation eliminates waiting by addressing students’ different needs for pacing, complexity, and challenge.
CTYI students reported infrequently being given differentiated lessons – lessons more challenging or more complex than the assignments of their peers. A majority (from 65% to 78%) of CTYI and CAT students reported rarely or never receiving differentiated lessons in their classes. It is possible that teachers provide differentiated assignments in a manner that is not obvious to students, but it would be difficult for them to not be aware so much of the time. Considering that nearly 85% of Irish secondary teachers reported they differentiate lessons for their high ability students (J. Cross et al., 2014), there appears to be a significant disconnect.
A regular concern among educators in gifted education is that their exceptionally able students will be bored by repetitive lessons aimed at their more average ability peers. According to students in Kanevsky and Keighley’s (2003) study of gifted high school dropouts, “(1) learning is the opposite of boredom, and (2) learning is the antidote to boredom” (p. 20). Among approximately half of CTYI students, boredom was most likely to occur from once a week to daily in their science, math, or English classes (see Figure 4.1). CAT students reported less frequent boredom in science and math classes, but a similar frequency of boredom in English. Irish and foreign language classes were less likely to be boring due to already knowing the material. These frequencies align with parents’ reports of children being frequently unchallenged in school, a major source of dissatisfaction with their children’s education (J. Cross, Cross et al., 2019). The picture painted in these studies of CTYI and CAT students is of infrequent differentiation, with quite a bit of redundancy in lessons.
To learn more about CTYI students’ experiences of school, twelve CTYI students (50% female) were interviewed in the summer of 2019. In general, the interviews confirmed the findings of the 2015 and 2016 studies: Students rarely receive differentiated lessons and are often bored in school. Several students gave examples of differentiation, of excellent teachers, and of a positive academic experience. Nearly all students, however, described being bored, having poor teachers or an unstimulating curriculum. Peers were an important feature of their school lives. Friends were frequently named first when asked what they think about when they think of school. As we saw in their survey responses, sometimes peers do get in the way of their learning. CTYI students have varied reactions to their peers’ slower learning, but in this study, they primarily viewed a deficient education as a result of curricular, teaching, or logistical breakdowns.
CTYI was a welcome change from school. One student was eloquent in his metaphorical description of the difference:
Sometimes it feels like when you’re in school it’s... Compared to here [CTYI], when I’m here it feels like I’m swimming in an ocean and you get this really hard wave coming at you and you have to really challenge yourself to get through it. But once you get through it, you’ve got the sort of nice atmospheric like relaxation where it’s swaying, you can smell the seaweed and the salt. You can taste it in your mouth. It’s a really nice moment. Where the education system is like a swimming pool. It’s just easy the entire way through. No challenge, but then there’s the stench of chlorine and almost as though nothing’s... It doesn’t feel very... It feels almost surreal or, yeah, it doesn’t feel natural. (M6)
Theme | Comment | Subtheme |
---|---|---|
Enjoyable Learning | I love science. I think we were learning about atomic structure and I was learning about the different subatomic particles, how everything interacts with each other, how different bonds form. And I found that very interesting…. I love language class as well. We were learning about, I think German in general. I find it very interesting. I’m rarely bored in German. There’s always something new to learn. New prepositions, sentence structure. (F5) | Novelty |
Well in maths, when we were doing, it was some, it was algebraic fractions. I kind of at the start I didn’t really understand them that much, so I felt it kind of challenging and I kind of liked that, because usually in maths I find it quite easy, and I kind of grasp it immediately. But then this time, I just couldn’t seem to grasp it straight off the bat. So I kind of liked doing that because I felt that it was more of a challenge and I got to work more and I wasn’t just bored of doing stuff. (F4) | Challenging | |
I really loved my science classes in second and third year. Because my teacher obviously really loved what she was doing. (F3) | Teacher | |
Well, I think TY is actually a pretty good example because there wasn’t much traditional classroom setting. So, there was an opportunity to actually leave school and attend other programs, and I did do that, and I worked with Concern for about a week, the charity, just learning what they do there, and that was really good. That was a really enjoyable learning experience. (F6) | TY | |
Not Learning | Sometimes, particularly in science and business, my teachers, they kind of like go very slow through what we’re doing on. It kind of gets quite boring because they kind of need to repeat everything multiple times and maybe we’d even, I can remember we were doing the circulatory system, and we took, like it was a short enough chapter, and then we took like, I think it was three or four weeks to get it all done. And it just felt kind of boring because it was very repetitive, and it was basically the same thing that they were saying all the time. (F4) | Pacing |
There are also times where there are other people in the class who just aren’t paying attention, and it’s forcing us to, say on maybe a second or third day, go over a certain topic again and again. I find that incredibly boring. (M3) | Peers | |
Our primary school covered a lot of Irish, way more Irish than most primary schools that my friends have been to. I don’t find Irish particularly interesting, but I learn it because I have to. But a lot of the times in Irish class or in math class, I find myself doing nothing, kind of doodling in my sketch book. (F5) | Irish Class | |
Challenge | I suppose if I have to actually think about something and work my way through it as opposed to just rote learning or this kind of copy and paste or just waffling. If I have to actually apply myself and use my brain. (F3) | Requires thinking |
Challenge is something I would associate with skill more so than anything. (M5) | Requires skill | |
Almost every subject will present some sort of challenge, some more than others, like math and English, I think, will present bigger challenges, and geography, at least for the junior cert course, is always been easy. (M1) | Requires specific | |
I’d say half and half as some subjects... Yeah, a lot of the subjects that are science lessons, the actual classes themselves, I don’t feel very challenged in them. The history classes, I don’t ever feel the challenge. I enjoy them because it’s something that I’m interested in. But I don’t feel challenged in them necessarily. (M6) | ||
Going in depth | I find science, I absolutely love it, but the teacher doesn’t go as fast or as in-depth as I would like her to. So, a lot of science I find myself staring blankly at the board or doodling while the teacher’s explaining something to the rest of the class and I’ve already got it down. I already understand it. Sometimes when I go home I would research more about it to learn more because if I find, especially the atomic structure, I will research more and learn more because I want to learn more. (F5) | Doesn’t happen |
Especially, I think, in science, they don’t go nearly as in-depth because what they’ve done is, now we’ve junior cert and junior cycle, because the courses changed. They didn’t change it all at once, so some subjects have changed and some haven’t. Science, for me, I was the first year of the new junior cycle, so they completely changed the course. One thing, what they’ve done is they’ve dumbed it down, basically, because a lot of people were struggling with science. They decided let’s make it really, really, really simple. You’ll have to learn loads of different topics, but you’ll only need to know this much, just scratch the surface. Whereas, I’d rather do core topics that you need to know but go in-depth with them. If you read our science book, you’d probably cry. It’s horrible. They only teach you about three or four organelles in a plant cell, when coming to CTY, you know that there’s way more. They won’t tell you that. (F1) | ||
I think I like going in depth things, in depth with things, particularly in subjects like history, or geography, or science, or business. I mean because you kind of... it kind of feels easier to understand why it’s happening. Like, say in science, for example, like reactions or something. Like if you just go over the very basics, you’re like, “Why is that happening?” But then when they go much more in depth it’s kind of easier to understand it. (F4) | Helps with learning | |
Good teachers | I think one really important thing is they command respect, that they don’t have to be a certain way in order for you to listen to them and to follow the rules, how they want you to act, and to be liked at the same time. I think that’s really important when they have to teach you something. (M3) | High expectations |
She does actually want you to learn and if you don’t do well, she does get a bit disappointed in you. I’ve always got on well with her and I’ve always had her as a maths teacher so I’ve liked her as a teacher. (F3) | ||
I suppose I’d define a good teacher as a teacher who can teach the lesson effectively and keep the class engaged, you know? If teacher is boring, then it’s their fault if the kids don’t listen. They have to hold them. (M5) | Interesting | |
She definitely was strict, but she knew how to teach. She would explain things to us, go over them, she would quiz us, she would make sure that every single person in the class knew what she was talking about. I don’t have a lot of teachers that do that. I find that that’s a really good teacher. She explains things in terms, breaks everything down. She goes quite in-depth, as well. I like her. She jokes quite a bit and she jumps around quite a bit. (F5) | ||
A teacher that values the efforts you put into their class and a teacher that values enthusiasm as well as knowledge. Also a teacher that can be open with people. Yeah, and a teacher that doesn’t try and hold you back or a teacher that doesn’t try and restrain your learning. (M6) | Accommodating | |
Well probably like someone that they kind of say, “Oh.” That they kind of understand that some... They’d understand that everyone works at different paces and they’d be able to facilitate both people who would be slower at grasping concepts and those who would be quicker. (F4) | ||
Bad teaching | And she takes ages doing something so you get bored in what’s you’re doing. We took months to read a book and you got bored of it, you know? When the teacher drags stuff out or it’s just always talking, you have to listen and listen (F2) | Boring |
my history teacher, I remember he told us a few things that were just kind of wrong. Like, I remember I’d look in the history book and just be ... You want one who knows the right answer. (M5) | Incompetent | |
She doesn’t explain things as well as much as she should. She is very adamant about her particular style of learning, and tries to push that onto the class, which tends to be write out of many pages of notes. Read, write again. (M1) | Poor strategies | |
This year, like higher level history, I think we watched like seven or eight movies all year, but that would be considered education. He’d be like, “So today we’re moving on to teaching of about 1960, and here’s a Michael Collins documentary, or here’s a Michael Collins movie,” whereas no one’s going to be able to sit down and watch a movie and take in the facts. They have to be taught, I would say anyway. So that style of teaching is just not viable. (M4) | ||
Boredom | I often find in school that I’m learning about things I already know or that a teacher dwells too long on a certain subject and I’m bored because I already understand it and the other students don’t. (F5) | Already know it |
It’s more so bored because of the delivery of the lesson, not more because I know it already. (F2) | Teaching style | |
Yeah, science, history, English. French sometimes. Maths. They’d be subjects where, yeah... Especially in science and history, I can get quite bored in subjects and I will say to myself... Like sometimes when I’m in science I get so bored I will get in trouble for simply talking to my friend or for doing something like that. But I just don’t see the appeal in learning about the respiratory system for the seventh time in the last term. …There’s a good few subjects I would feel bored in because we’ve covered it before. (M6). | Subject specific | |
But then there are also times where there are other people in the class who are just aren’t paying attention, and it’s forcing us to, say on maybe a second or third day, go over a certain | Other students | |
Climate | I find the whole environment of my school is not something I particularly want to be a part of. It was the school nearest to me, so I’m going there. Like I said, with the teachers, you don’t particularly feel included and with a lot of student life. It can be quite difficult because there’s just a lot of people who aren’t particularly focused on school, we’ll say. (M4) | Nonacademic |
I think of people being loud and obnoxious. I suppose I read a lot in school, as well. (M5) | ||
I like the social aspect of school and then some classes as well. Not all of them. Generally positive like… There’s just a friendly atmosphere. (F3) | Good social experience | |
I think most of the time the education part of school just kind of goes by in a blur, and it’s only really the socializing that I remember. Because socializing, I mean every day it’s something new. Learning, not all the time. (F5) |
A dramatic shift in CTYI students’ experience of school occurred in the spring of 2020. The emergence of the COVID-19 virus led to the shuttering of businesses, travel, and schools. During the pandemic, most education across Ireland and the world moved to a virtual platform, at least for some period of time. School was likely to be very different for CTYI students, not just socially, as described in Chapter 3, but also academically. In the summer of 2021, CTYI was fully in session, with all courses offered virtually. Students had been in virtual school early in the pandemic, but most students had moved to in-person school by this time. We took this opportunity to ask CTYI students about their pandemicera educational experiences. What were their experiences like in online school, in in-person classes, and how was that different from their experiences of CTYI’s online courses? More than 300 students participated in the online survey in the summer of 2021.
CTYI students reported that teachers more frequently made time for students’ questions (the Teacher Support factor; see Figure 4.2) in in-person classes than online. Teachers more frequently motivated their students and classes were more interesting, challenging, and fun (the Motivation Support factor) in in-person classes than in online classes. Students more regularly believed they could work at a higher level and keep up with their learning, or manage their time (the My Learning factor) when in-person than online. In contrast to these more positive in-person experiences, students reported they were better able to access a computer or the internet when needed in online than in in-person classes. In general, however, CTYI students perceived a better learning environment in their in-person classes, with the possible exception of computer access in school. CTYI’s virtual classes had even greater teacher availability and support for students’ motivation than in-person classes.
Chapter 5
In 2017, partners in two countries were interested in collaborating on the research being conducted at CTYI – the Center for Talented Youth-Greece (CTYG), at Anatolia College in Thessalonika, and the Jagadis Bose National Science Talent Search (JBNS) in Kolkata. CTYG employs a definition similar to CTYI, accepting students scoring in the 95th percentile on a standardized ability test.
JBNS participants were attending the Talent Search and Innovation in Science Pursuit for Inspired Research (INSPIRE) programs. Admission to the programs requires a top 1% score on the national board examinations or through aptitude testing and interviews. The survey used in the 2015 study was modified for the Greek and Indian contexts and data was collected in 2017 and 2018.
The CTYG students were evenly divided by gender (50.7% female). The majority of JBNS students were male (63.5%). Students in the JBNS sample were almost exclusively in the grade equivalent of 5th Year, whereas CTYG students were nearly all 1st through 3rd year. The comparison CTYI and CAT samples were more evenly distributed among males and females and by Junior and Senior cycle.
The personality measure was not available for CTYG students, but JBNS students could be compared with Irish students on the personality measure. The three samples – CTYI, CAT, and JBNS – were similar in Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness. JBNS students were more extraverted and more agreeable than CTYI students. Self-efficacy scores were available for all four samples. CTYG students, who tended to be younger than the students in the other countries, had self-efficacy scores that were consistently higher than the Irish students’ scores, with the exception of self-efficacy to Resist Peer Pressure. JBNS students tended to have lower self-efficacy scores than CTYI and CAT students, but they were similar to CAT students in their Social and Self-Regulated Learning self efficacy. Implicit Person Theory was similar among the four groups. All had median scores bordering on a fixed mindset, hovering around a 3. All scores were closer to an incremental mindset than a fixed one.
Students in the four countries varied most in their social cognitive beliefs (Figure 5.1) and response to the scenarios (Figure 5.2). CTYI students had a notably high preference for working independently and higher agreement that other students get in the way of their learning. CTYG and JBNS students were less likely to agree they get more quickly bored with small talk than peers or that other students get in the way of their learning. All students agree at least somewhat that they are more serious about learning and prefer to work independently.
The variability of responses to the scenarios, which carried different threats of exposure of their giftedness (see Chapter 3), followed the same pattern in all four programs (Figure 5.2). There was more truth-telling and placating in the two scenarios with lower threat, Onomatopoeia and Substitute Teacher. As in other studies of the scenarios, there was a greater spread of responses along the spectrum for the Biology Exam scenario. JBNS and CTYG students preferred the Placate response when the threat was low for their giftedness to be exposed, whereas only about one third of Irish students chose Placate. JBNS students were more likely than the other students to choose the Lie option for the low threat scenario. In the high-threat Biology Exam scenario, fewer JBNS students than expected chose more truthful options, preferring the Preface no answer option. JBNS students were less likely to choose Lie than CTYI and CAT students (13.7% vs 28.4% and 23.5%, respectively). Very few of the CTYG students (5.5%) chose the Lie option in the Biology Exam scenario.
There may be cultural reasons for these differences, but development may also play a role. The younger CTYG students may not yet have faced situations where more truthful answers negatively affect their social latitude. The higher agreeableness among JBNS students may explain their preference for avoiding the truth, which may be hurtful for their peers to hear. Ostracism scores were similar among CTYI, CAT, and JBNS students, but CTYG students reported being less ostracized. CTYG students felt less ignored and excluded from their peers than did the students in the other countries. This pattern held true even when comparing the younger sample of CTYG students with the same-aged students in the CTYI and CAT programs.
Students in Ireland were more likely than students in the other two countries to report that their assignments were rarely or never differentiated to target their ability level. JBNS students indicated they more regularly received differentiated assignments. Students in all programs reported being bored once a week or more often in their science classes. CTYG students were more frequently bored in their math and English classes than students in the other programs. JBNS students were unlikely to report being frequently bored in history or geography lessons.
Chapter 6
The research described in this report has the potential to affect Irish gifted students’ well-being and improve the opportunities for the maximization of their potential. When we better understand their psychology and their social and academic experiences, we have an opportunity to strengthen their foundations of support at home and in school.
It is of paramount importance that others do not view them as monolithic, fitting a stereotype developed from media portrayals or experience with a few outliers recognizable for their intellectual talents. There is great diversity in this population, but the exceptional abilities they share may result in similar experiences in their environments. Attention to their internal differences and the effects these may have on their lived experiences can help adults fashion supportive environments.
The personality types identified among CTYI students are similar to those found in the general population, although the finding of a High Resilient group is a significant difference, as is the higher conscientiousness among students in the Undercontroller class. Personality is considered stable, but it is not impervious to change. In fact, studies have found less stability among adolescents, with truly stable personality occurring only in one’s 40’s. The CTYI students in our study may have a different personality profile as they mature. The majority of students fit the resilient type and will be able to adapt to most settings.
Gifted adolescents in general tend to be less extraverted than their typical peers. The CTYI students who are more introverted will flourish in calm, small group settings that attend to their introversion by reducing irritating stimuli. Those who tend to be less emotionally stable (high neuroticism) will benefit when others encourage their resilience through caring and supportive messaging. Professional counseling may be needed to help the Overcontroller students in reshaping their concerns of being evaluated by others (socially prescribed perfectionism) and providing strategies for coping with stressful situations. CTYI students in the Undercontroller class will benefit from social skill development, including development of their perspective-taking skills. Their strong desire to engage with others (extraversion) may provide an inroad to teaching better strategies for interaction.
CTYI students tended to have strong confidence in their academic abilities, although this did vary by subject area. The students in the Superstars class, who made up 25% of the students in the 2013-2105 studies, had very high confidence across the board, but nearly all the other students could improve on their belief that they can enlist the support of parents, siblings, and community members to help them with a problem. Teaching students how to recognize the sources of support they can count on and how to persuasively articulate their needs may improve their beliefs about others’ support for them. In other areas where confidence is lacking, students will benefit when they have opportunities to be successful, particularly after receiving constructive feedback, or seeing the model of others trying hard at a task. Boosting self-efficacy will improve students’ well-being and their academic success. The importance of authentic praise, for students’ actual behaviors and not just the end result, is helpful in developing self-efficacy and a growth mindset.
Nearly all CTYI students exhibited high levels of selforiented perfectionism. This type of perfectionism is associated with positive striving, a good thing in terms of academic success and well-being. When CTYI students become concerned that they must be perfect to meet others’ expectations, negative outcomes are likely to ensue. To avoid this concern, which was highest among the students in the Overcontroller class, it is important to foster an ethic of care, reducing unrealistic expectations. Knowing what is unrealistic for these students can be a challenge in itself, because they can do so much. In an atmosphere of trust, when they are not worried about being dismissed or criticized, these students will tell you what they think. Listening to them, encouraging them to be open with their feedback, will be successful only if adults are committed to being responsive to the students’ needs. Greenspon (2021) recommends adults develop empathy for the student by attempting to learn how they see the world. Pointing out their likeable qualities, as opposed to their achievements, can draw attention away from the perfect products and behaviors the students have come to believe is so important. High expectations alone do not produce fearful perfectionists. When they are accompanied by adults who model a positive attitude toward failure as a learning opportunity, who are warm and accepting of the child’s efforts, students will lose their fear of being evaluated negatively.
The majority of CTYI students in this research had indicators of positive peer relationships. These students are like many others who feel different from peers, especially in terms of their seriousness about learning. This is one reason programs like CTYI are so important. Students are able to find intellectual peers who are similarly motivated to learn. In a mixed-ability classroom, CTYI students may worry about the visibility of their exceptional cognitive abilities. They may be concerned about hurting their peers’ feelings if they outperform them. Teachers who avoid talking about the academic hierarchy in the class (e.g., holding up one student’s work as an example, pointing out who performed best) will reduce the cost of outperformance (Mikami et al., 2012).
Students can learn strategies for maintaining positive relationships with peers who cannot perform as well. Most already know the strategies of lowering oneself (e.g., hiding their accomplishments, downplaying their success, etc.) and helping peers, as we learned in these studies. Other strategies that can be helpful are likely to be effective in supporting a mixed-ability relationship: simply being nice, complimenting the other person, and doing favors. A positive environment will be a natural outcome when all students are encouraged to engage in prosocial behaviors. Lowering oneself, while helpful to relationships in the moment, may result in underachievement and loss of opportunity.
An appropriate education for gifted students is one that has curriculum that is more challenging than average, which utilizes advanced materials with options for learning at greater breadth, depth, and level of abstraction, offered at a pace that matches their rate of learning (Tomlinson, 2005). Finding the right combination of these characteristics for each child requires significant teacher skill, time for planning, and access to resources. High percentages of CTYI students reported rarely or never receiving assignments more challenging or complex than their peers received, indicating that the 85% of Irish teachers in a 2014 study (J. Cross et al., 2014) who claimed to be differentiating instruction in 51 their classes were likely not doing so effectively. Many CTYI students reported being frequently bored in their classes and unable to go as in-depth as they would like.
Effective differentiation is only possible with appropriate professional development for teachers, adequate time for increased lesson planning, and the materials needed for this specialized instruction. All of this requires strong support from administrators. The contemporary movement in gifted education is toward a talent development approach (National Association for Gifted Children Talent Development Task Force, 2015), which takes a broad, inclusive perspective to offer advanced instruction to all students capable of achieving at that level. The focus is on providing opportunities to explore domains of talent early, with targeted instruction designed to develop students’ abilities in areas where they show exceptional ability and are motivated to learn at a higher level than peers. Talent development as the framework for offering gifted education will be best accomplished when the whole school applies its principles (T. Cross & Cross, 2021a).
CTYI programs offer gifted students tremendous opportunities for enrichment in a stimulating, challenging environment. When gifted education is lacking in their home schools, CTYI students find great satisfaction in the advanced programs it offers and revel in the time spent with intellectual peers. CTYI does an outstanding job of attending to the needs of this population, but it is available only to students who can access classes at the Dublin City University campus or other satellite locations and who can afford the tuition. Making it more available to students across the country and providing scholarships to those who qualify would be a great boon to more gifted students in Ireland.
Enrichment programs like CTYI, motivating and challenging as they are, are not a substitute for a home school that addresses gifted students’ needs. Advanced curriculum should be planned, with a scope and sequence that can be applied throughout the 13 years of schooling. To become expert in students’ areas of talent, they must have a firm foundation for their later learning, with increasing challenge that leads to expert performance. Additionally, schools must attend to the psychosocial needs of their students, without which they are unlikely to persevere through difficulty or know the psychological and social strategies needed to succeed in their talent domain. Trained school counselors who are versed in the unique needs of gifted students and how to offer the support they need will be invaluable (T. Cross & Cross, 2021b).
Irish gifted students are not all alike. They differ in personality, confidence, social acumen, and interests. Among the students attending CTYI programs who participated in these studies were many confident, well-adjusted, and socially competent adolescents. There was also a subset of students who need extra support from adults who care about them and their well-being. It is important that we better understand these students and learn how to create environments that allow them to achieve their maximum potential while living a good life.
Ablard, K. E., & Mills, C. J. (1996). Implicit theories of intelligence and self-perceptions of academically talented adolescents and children. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 25, 137–148. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537340
Adams, C. (2021). Introduction to programming for gifted learners. In T. L. Cross & J. R. Cross (Eds.) Handbook for counselors serving students with gifts and talents, 2nd ed. (pp. 125-143). Prufrock Academic Press.
Aguilar, L., Downey, G., Krauss, R., Pardo, J., Lane, S., & Bolger, N. (2016). A dyadic perspective on speech accommodation and social connection: Both partners’ rejection sensitivity matters. Journal of Personality, 84(2), 165–177. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12149
Almack, J. C. (1922). The influence of intelligence on the selection of associates. School Sociology, 16, 529–30.
Altemeyer, R. A. (1998). The other “authoritarian personality.” In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 47–92). Academic Press.
American Psychological Association. (2020). APA dictionary of psychology. https://dictionary.apa.org
Asendorpf, J. B., Borkenau, P., Ostendorpf, F., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2001). Carving personality description at its joints: Confirmation of three replicable personality prototypes for both child and adults. European Journal of Personality, 15, 169–198.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social Learning Theory. Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1989). Multidimensional scales of perceived self-efficacy [Database Record]. PsycTests. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/t06802-000
Bandura A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The exercise of control. Freeman.
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1-26.
Barańczuk, U. (2021). The Five-Factor Model of personality and generalized self-efficacy: A meta-analysis. Journal of Individual Differences, 42(4), 183–193. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000345 (Supplemental)
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529.
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology, 4, 99–102.
Bellmore, A. (2011). Peer rejection and unpopularity: Associations with GPAs across the transition to middle school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(2), 282–295. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023312
Berlin, J. E. (2009). It’s all a matter of perspective: Student perceptions on the impact of being labeled gifted and talented. Roeper Review, 31(4), 217–223. https://doi.org/10.1080/02783190903177580
Blackwell, L., Trzesniewski, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78(1), 246–263.
Bratt, C., Sidanius, J., & Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (2016). Shaping the development of prejudice: Latent growth modeling of the influence of social dominance orientation on outgroup affect in youth. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(12), 1617–1634. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167216666267
Callahan, C. M., Moon, T. R., & Oh, S. (2017). Describing the status of programs for the gifted: a call for action. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 40(1) 20–49. https://doi.org/10.1177/0162353216686215
Carter-Sowell, A. R., Wesselmann, E. D., Wirth, J. H., Law, A., Zhansheng Chen, Kosasih, M. W., van der Lee, R., & Williams, K. D. (2010). Strides for belonging trump strides for superiority: Effects of being ostracized for being superior or inferior to the others. Journal of Individual Psychology, 66(1), 68–92.
Cohen, R., Duncan, M., & Cohen, S. L. (1994). Classroom peer relations of children participating in a pull-out enrichment program. Gifted Child Quarterly, 38(1), 33–37. https://doi.org/10.1177/001698629403800105
Coleman, L. J. (1985). Schooling the gifted. Addison-Wesley.
Coleman, L. J., & Cross, T. L. (1988). Is being gifted a social handicap? Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 11, 41-56.
Coleman, L. J., & Cross, T. L. (1993). Relationships between practices and outcomes in a summer residential school for gifted adolescents. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 16(4), 420-441.
Coleman, L. J., Cross, T. L., & Cross, J. R. (2021). Lived experience, mixed messages and stigma. In T. L. Cross & J. R. Cross (Eds.) Handbook for Counselors Serving Students with Gifts and Talents, 2nd ed. (pp. 427-450). Prufrock Academic Press.
Coleman, L. J., Micko, K. J., & Cross, T. L. (2015). Twenty-five years of research on the lived experience of being gifted in school: Capturing the students’ voices. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 38, 358–376. https://doi.org/10.1177/0162353215607322
Costa, A., & Faria, L. (2018). Implicit theories of intelligence and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review. Frontiers of Psychology, 9, 829. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00829
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101.
Cross, J. R. (2016). Crowds. In R. J. R. Levesque (Ed.) Encyclopedia of Adolescence (pp. 573-580). Springer.
Cross, J. R. (2021). Peer relationships. In T. L. Cross & J. R. Cross (Eds.) Handbook for Counselors Serving Students with Gifts and Talents, 2nd ed. (pp. 471-488). Prufrock Press.
Cross, J. R., & Cross, T. L. (2015). Clinical and mental health issues in counseling the gifted individual. Journal of Counseling & Development, 93, 163-172.
Cross, J. R., Bugaj, S. J., & Mammadov, S. (2016). Accepting a scholarly identity: Gifted students, academic crowd membership, and identification with school. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 39, 23-48. https://doi.org/10.1177/0162353215624162
Cross, J. R., Cross, T. L., O’Reilly, C., & Mammadov, S. (2014). Gifted Education in Ireland: Educators’ Beliefs and Practices. Report prepared for Centre for Talented Youth – Ireland.
Cross, J. R., Cross, T. L., O’Reilly, C., Vaughn, C. T., & Carroll, E. (2019). Gifted education in Ireland: Parents’ beliefs and experiences. Report prepared for Centre for Talented Youth – Ireland.
Cross, J. R., & Fletcher, K. L. (2010). Associations of parental and peer characteristics with adolescents’ social dominance orientation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 40, 694-706. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-010-9585-7
Cross, J. R., & Fletcher, K. L. (2017, August). Comparison of Social Dominance Orientation between Honors and General College Students. Poster presentation at the annual convention of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
Cross, J. R., O’Reilly, C., Kim, M., Mammadov, S. & Cross, T. L. (2015). Social coping and self-concept among young gifted students in Ireland and the United States: A cross-cultural study. High Ability Studies, 26, 39-61. https://doi.org/10.1080/13598139.2015.1031881
Cross, J. R., Vaughn, C. T., Mammadov, S., Cross, T. L., Kim, M., O’Reilly, C., Spielhagen, F., Pereira Da Costa, M., & Hymer, B. (2019). A cross-cultural study of the social experience of giftedness. Roeper Review, 41, 224-242.
Cross, T. L., & Coleman, L. J. (1992). Gifted high school students’ advice to science teachers. Gifted Child Today, 15(5), 25-26.
Cross, T. L., Coleman, L. J., & Stewart, R. A. (1993). The social cognition of gifted adolescents: An exploration of the stigma of giftedness paradigm. Roeper Review, 16(1), 37-40.
Cross, T. L., Coleman, L. J., & Stewart, R. A. (1995). Psychosocial diversity of gifted adolescents: An exploration of the stigma of the giftedness paradigm. Roeper Review, 17, 181–185.
Cross, T. L., Coleman, L. J., & Terhaar-Yonkers, M. (1991). The social cognition of gifted adolescents in schools: Managing the stigma of giftedness. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 15, 44–55.
Cross, T. L., & Cross, J. R. (2017). Suicide among students with gifts and talents. In S. Pfeiffer, E. Shaunessy-Dedrick, & M. Foley-Nicpon (Eds.), APA handbook of giftedness and talent (pp. 601–614). American Psychological Association.
Cross, T. L., & Cross, J. R. (2021a). A school-based conception of giftedness: Clarifying roles and responsibilities in the development of talent in our public schools. In R. J. Sternberg & D. Ambrose (Eds.) Conceptions of Giftedness and Talent (pp. 83-98). Palgrave MacMillan.
Cross, T. L., & Cross, J. R. (Eds.). (2021b). Handbook for Counselors Serving Students with Gifts and Talents, 2nd ed. Prufrock Press.
Cross, T. L., & Cross, J. R. (2022). From managing stigma to forming friendships: Introducing a model of friendship development. Gifted Child Today, 45(1), 58-61. DOI: 10.1177/10762175211050697.
Cross, T. L., Cross, J. R., & O’Reilly, C. (2018). Attitudes about gifted education among Irish educators. High Ability Studies, 29(2), 169-189.
Cross, T. L., Hernandez, N. R., & Coleman, L. J. (1991). Governor’s schools: An idea whose time has come. Gifted Child Today, 14(3), 29-31.
Dai, D. Y. (2004). How universal is the big-fish-little-pond effect? American Psychologist, 59, 267–268.
Dai, D. Y., & Rinn, A. N. (2008). The big-fish–little-pond effect: What do we know and where do we go from here? Educational Psychological Review, 20, 283-317.
Damian, L. E., Stoeber, J., Negru, S. O., & Băban, A. (2017). On the development of perfectionism: The longitudinal role of academic achievement and academic efficacy. Journal of Personality, 85(4), 565–577. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12261
De Vries, R. E., Pronk, J., Olthof, T., & Goossens, F. A. (2020). Getting along and/or getting ahead: Differential hexaco personality correlates of likeability and popularity among adolescents. European Journal of Personality. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2243
Deci, E. L. & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268.
Department of Education. (2021). Education indicators for Ireland December 2021. https://www.gov.ie/en/publication/055810-education-statistics/
Does, S., & Mentovich, A. (2016). Rooting for the top dog: How social dominance motives shape group preference in intergroup competition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 62, 24–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.09.002
Donellan, M. B., & Robins, R. W. (2010). Resilient, overcontrolled, and undercontrolled personality types: Issues and controversies. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 1070–1083.
Duckitt, J. (2001). A dual-process cognitive-motivational theory of ideology and prejudice. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 33, pp. 41–113). Academic Press.
Duffy, A. L., Gardner, A. A., & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2020). Peer rejection and dislike. In M. J. Zimmer-Gembeck & A. M. Waters, (Volume Editors) S. Hupp & J. D. Jewel, (Series Eds.), The Encyclopedia of Child and Adolescent Development: Emotion in Childhood (pp. 1727–1740). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/ 9781119171492.wecad191
Dweck, C. S. (1975). The role of expectations and attributions in the alleviation of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 674–685. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0077149
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality and development. Taylor & Francis.
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset. Random House.
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256-273.
Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., & Mac Iver, D. (1993). Development during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment fit on young adolescents' experiences in schools and in families. American Psychologist, 48, 90-101.
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) and S. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (5th ed., pp. 1017–1095). Wiley.
Eisenberger, N. J., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K., D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290-292.
Ekehammar, B., Akrami, N., Gylje, M., Zakrisson, I. (2004). What matters most to prejudice: Big five personality, social dominance orientation, or right-wing authoritarianism? European Journal of Personality, 18, 463-482.
Exline, J. J., & Lobel, M. (1999). The perils of outperformance: Sensitivity about being the target of a threatening upward comparison. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 307–337. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.307
Farmer, T. W., & Hollowell, J. H. (1994). Social networks in mainstream classrooms: Social affiliations and behavioral characteristics of students with EBD. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 2(3), 143–155, 163. https://doi.org/10.1177/1 06342669400200302
Farmer, T., Irvin, M., Leung, M.-C., Hall, C., Hutchins, B., & McDonough, E. (2010). Social preference, social prominence, and group membership in late elementary school: homophilic concentration and peer affiliation configurations. Social Psychology of Education, 13(2), 271–293. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-009-9107-1
Fegert, J. M., Vitiello, B., Plener, P. L., & Clemens, V. (2020). Challenges and burden of the Coronavirus 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic for child and adolescent mental health: A narrative review to highlight clinical and research needs in the acute phase and the long return to normality. Child & Adolescent Psychiatry & Mental Health, 14(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-020-00329-3
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872675400700202
Finn J. D., & Zimmer K. S. (2012). Student engagement: What is it? Why does it matter? In Christenson S., Reschly A., Wylie C. (Eds.) Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 97-131). Springer.
Fletcher, K. L., & Speirs Neumeister, K. L. (2012). Research on perfectionism and achievement motivation: Implications for gifted students. Psychology in the Schools, 49, 668–677. doi:10.1002/pits.21623
Fletcher, K. L., & Neumeister, K. S. (2017). Perfectionism in schools: When achievement is not so perfect. Momentum Press.
Foliano F., Rolfe H., Buzzeo J., Runge J. & Wilkinson D. (2019). Changing mindsets: Effectiveness trial. Evaluation Report. Education Endowment Foundation, London. https://www.niesr.ac.uk/sites/default/files/publications/Changing%20Min…;
Francis, B., Skelton, C., & Read, B. (2010). The simultaneous production of educational achievement and popularity: How do some pupils accomplish it? British Educational Research Journal, 36, 317–340.
Freitag, E. (2020, October 30). Letʼs fight about the future of public education. Chalkbeat. https://www.chalkbeat.org/2020/10/30/21540098/lets-fight-future-of-public-education
French, L. R., Walker, C. L., & Shore, B. M. (2011). Do gifted students really prefer to work alone? Roeper Review, 33, 145–159.
Gadzikowski, A. (2016). Everyday differentiation: How administrators support differentiation of curriculum and instruction in early childhood classrooms. Exchange, January/February, 12-16. www.ChildCareExchange.com
Gilman, R., Carter-Sowell, A., DeWall, C. N., Adams, R. E., & Carboni, I. (2013). Validation of the Ostracism Experience Scale for Adolescents. Psychological Assessment, 25(2), 319–330. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030913
Gorman, A. H., Kim, J., & Schimmelbusch, A. (2002). The attributes adolescents associate with peer popularity and teacher preference. Journal of School Psychology, 40, 143–165.
Greenspon, T. S. (2021). Perfectionism in context: Empathic gateways to a recovery process. In T. L. Cross & J. R. Cross (Eds.) Handbook for Counselors Serving Students with Gifts and Talents, 2nd ed. (pp. 733-752). Prufrock Academic Press.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2017). The implicit revolution: Reconceiving the relation between conscious and unconscious. American Psychologist, 72(9), 861–871. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000238
Guo, G. (2006). Genetic similarity shared by best friends among adolescents. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 113-121.
Hakulinen, C., Elovainio, M., Pulkki, R. L., Virtanen, M., Kivimäki, M., & Jokela, M. (2015). Personality and depressive symptoms: Individual participant meta-analysis of 10 cohort studies. Depression & Anxiety (1091-4269), 32(7), 461–470. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22376
Hamm, J. V. (2000). Do birds of a feather flock together? The variable bases for African American, Asian American, and European American adolescents’ selection of similar friends. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 209–219. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012- 1649.36.2.209
Hartl, A. C., Laursen, B., Cantin, S., & Vitaro, F. (2020). A test of the bistrategic control hypothesis of adolescent popularity. Child Development, 91(3), e635–e648. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13269
Heaven, P. C. L., & Bucci, S. (2001). Right-wing authoritarianism, social dominance orientation and personality: An analysis using the IPIP measure. European Journal of Personality, 15, 49–56.
Heaven, P. C. L., Ciarrochi, J., & Leeson, P. (2011). Cognitive ability, right-wing authoritarianism, and social dominance orientation: A five-year longitudinal study amongst adolescents. Intelligence, 39, 15-21.
Hébert, T. P. (2001). “If I had a new notebook, I know things would change”: Bright under- achieving young men in urban classrooms. Gifted Child Quarterly, 45, 174-194. https://doi.org/10.1177/001698620104500303
Herek, G. M. (2016). A nuanced view of stigma for understanding and addressing sexual and gender minority health disparities. LGBT Health, 3, 397–399. https://doi.org/10.1089/lgbt.2016.0154
Hermann, K. M., & Lawrence, C. (2012). Family relationships. T. L. Cross & J. R. Cross (Eds.) Handbook for counselors serving students with gifts and talents (pp. 393-407). Prufrock Press.
Hertberg-Davis, H. (2009). Myth 7: Differentiation in the regular classroom is equivalent to gifted programs and is sufficient: Classroom teachers have the time, the skill, and the will to differentiate adequately. Gifted Child Quarterly, 53(4), 251-253. https://doi.org/10.1177/0016986209346927
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456 – 470.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (2004). Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale: Technical manual. Multi-Health Systems.
Hinch, L., Cross, J. R., Cross, T. L., & O’Reilly, C. (2018, August). Differentiation and teacher efficacy in Irish classrooms. Presentation at the European Council for High Ability, Dublin, Ireland.
Ho, A. K., Sidanius, J., Pratto, F., Levin, S., Thomsen, L., Kteily, N., & Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (2012). Social dominance orientation: Revisiting the structure and function of a variable predicting social and political attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(5), 583–606. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211432765
Huang, L.-L., & Liu, J. H. (2005). Personality and social structural implications of the situational priming of social dominance orientation. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(2), 267–276.
Huey, S. J., Jr., & Weisz, J. R. (1997). Ego control, ego resiliency, and the Five-Factor Model as predictors of behavioral and emotional problems in clinic-referred children and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(3), 404–415. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.106.3.404
John, O. P., Angleitner, A., & Ostendorf, F. (1988). The lexical approach to personality: A historical review of trait taxonomic research. European Journal of Personality, 2, 171-203.
John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five Inventory–versions 4a and 54. Berkeley: Institute of Personality and Social Research, University of California, Berkeley.
Jost, J. T., & Thompson, E. P. (2000). Group-based dominance and opposition to equality as independent predictors of self-esteem, ethnocentrism, and social policy attitudes among African Americans and European Americans. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 209-232.
Kandler, C. (2012). Nature and Nurture in Personality Development: The Case of Neuroticism and Extraversion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(5), 290–296. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412452557
Kanevsky, L., & Keighley, T. (2003). To produce or not to produce? Understanding boredom and the honor in underachievement. Roeper Review, 26, 20–28.
King, R. B. (2020). Mindsets are contagious: The social contagion of implicit theories of intelligence among classmates. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(2), 349–363. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12285
Larson, R., & Richards, M. H. (1991). Daily companionship in late childhood and early adolescence: Changing developmental contexts. Child Development. 62, 284–300.
Lee, S-Y., Olszewski-Kubilius, P., & Thomson, D. T. (2012). Academically gifted students’ perceived interpersonal competence and peer relationships. Gifted Child Quarterly, 56, 90-104.
Lippa, R., & Arad, S. (1999). Gender, personality, and prejudice: The display of authoritarianism and social dominance in interviews with college men and women. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 463–493.
Locke, K. D. (2006). What predicts well-being: A consistent self-concept or a desirable self- concept? Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 25, 228–247.
London, B., Downey, G., Bonica, C., & Paltin, I. (2007). Social causes and consequences of rejection sensitivity. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17(3), 481–506. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2007.00531.x
Lyon, K. A., Elliott R., Ware, K., Juhasz, G., & Brown L. J. E. (2021). Associations between facets and aspects of big five personality and affective disorders: A systematic review and best evidence synthesis.” Journal of Affective Disorders, 288, 175–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.03.061.
Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationships: A developmental account. American Psychologist, 45, 513-520.
Maddux, J. E. (Ed.). (1995). Self-efficacy, adaptation, and adjustment: Theory, research, and application. Plenum Press.
Mammadov, S. (2020). A comparison of creativity‐relevant personal characteristics in adolescents across personality profiles. The Journal of Creative Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.451
Mammadov, S. (2021). The Big Five personality traits and academic performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality. Advanced Online Publication. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12663
Mammadov, S., Ward, T. J., Cross, J. R., & Cross, T. L. (2016). Use of latent profile analysis in studies of gifted students. Roeper Review, 38, 175-184. DOI: 10.1080/02783193.2016.1183739
Marques, M. D., Feather, N. T., Austin, D. E. J., & Sibley, C. G. (2022). Attitudes towards favoring the fall of tall poppies: The role of social dominance orientation, authoritarianism, political ideologies, and self-esteem. The Journal of Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2021.1944034
Marsh, H. W. (1990). The Self-Description Questionnaire 1: Manual and research monograph. New South Wales, Australia: University of Western Sydney, McArthur.
Marsh, H. W., & Ayotte, V. (2003). Do multiple dimensions of self-concept become more differentiated with age? The differential distinctiveness hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 687–706.
Marsh, H. W., Chessor, D., Craven, R., & Roche, L. (1995). The effects of gifted and talented programs on academic self-concept: The big fish strikes again. American Educational Research Journal, 32, 285– 319.
Marsh, H. W., & Hau, K. T. (2003). Big-fish-little-pond effect on academic self-concept: A cross-cultural (26-country) test of the negative effects of academically selective schools. American Psychologist, 58, 364–376.
Marsh, H. W., & Shavelson, R. J. (1985). Self-concept: Its multifaceted, hierarchical structure. Educational Psychologist, 20, 107–123.
Marsh, H. W., & Yeung, A. (1997). Coursework selection: Relations to academic self- concept and achievement. American Educational Research Journal, 34, 691–720.
Martin, L. T., Burns, R. M., & Schonlau, M. (2010). Mental disorders among gifted and nongifted youth: A selected review of the epidemiologic literature. Gifted Child Quarterly, 54, 31–41. doi:10.1177/0016986209352684
Maslow, A. H. (1958). A Dynamic Theory of Human Motivation. In C. L. Stacey & M. DeMartino (Eds.), Understanding human motivation (pp. 26–47). Howard Allen Publishers. https://doi.org/10.1037/11305-004
Matthews, M. S. (2014). Self-concept and the gifted. In J. A. Plucker & C. M. Callahan (Eds.), Critical issues and practices in gifted education: What the research says., 2nd ed. (pp. 567–575). Prufrock Press
Mayer, R. E. (2011). Applying the science of learning. Pearson Merrill Prentice Hall.
McAdams, D. P. (2019). The emergence of personality. In D. P. McAdams, R. L. Shiner, & J. L. Tackett (Eds.), Handbook of personality development, pp. 3-19. Guilford Press.
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., Terracciano, A., Parker, W. D., Mills, C. J., De Fruyt, F., & Mervielde, I. (2002). Personality trait development from age 12 to age 18: Longitudinal, cross-sectional, and cross-cultural analyses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1456–1468.
McDougall, P., Hymel, S., Vaillancourt, T., & Mercer, L. (2001). The consequences of childhood peer rejection. In M. R. Leary (Ed.), Interpersonal rejection (pp. 213–247). Oxford University Press.
Mikami, A. Y., Griggs, M. S., Reuland, M. M., & Gregory, A. (2012). Teacher practices as predictors of children’s classroom social preference. Journal of School Psychology, 50, 95-111.
Milfont, T. L., Bain, P. G., Kashima, Y., Corral-Verdugo, V., Pasquali, C., Johansson, L.-O., Guan, Y., Gouveia, V. V., Garðarsdóttir, R. B., Doron, G., Bilewicz, M., Utsugi, A., Aragones, J. I., Steg, L., Soland, M., Park, J., Otto, S., Demarque, C., Wagner, C., … Einarsdóttir, G. (2018). On the relation between social dominance orientation and environmentalism: A 25-nation study. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(7), 802–814. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617722832
Mills, C. J. & Parker, W. D. (1998). Cognitive-psychological profiles of gifted adolescents from Ireland and the U.S.: Cross-societal comparisons. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 22(1), 1–16.
Mio, M. G., & Matsumuto, Y. (2018). A single-session universal mental health promotion program in Japanese schools: A pilot study. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 46(10), 1727–1743. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.7157
Miu, A. S., & Yeager, D. S. (2015). Preventing symptoms of depression by teaching adolescents that people can change: Effects of a brief incremental theory of personality intervention at 9-month follow-up. Clinical Psychological Science, 3, 726–743.
Mofield, E. L., & Chakraborti-Ghosh, S. (2010). Addressing multidimensional perfectionism in gifted adolescents with affective curriculum. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 33(4), 479–513. https://doi.org/10.1177/016235321003300403.
Mofield, E. L., & Parker Peters, M. (2018). Mindset misconception? Comparing mindsets, perfectionism, and attitudes of achievement in gifted, advanced, and typical students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 62(4), 327–349. doi https://doi.org/10.1177/0016986218758440.
National Association for Gifted Children Talent Development Task Force. (2015). Report to the Board. https://www.nagc.org/sites/default/files/Governance/TalentDevelopmentTFReport_11%2003%2015_FINAL.pdf
Neihart, M., & Olenchak, F. R. (2002). Creatively gifted children. In M. Neihart, S. M. Reis, N. M. Robinson, & S. M. Moon (Eds.), The social and emotional development of gifted children: What do we know? (pp. 165–176). Prufrock Press.
Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Children’s peer relations: a meta-analytic review of popular, rejected, neglected, controversial, and average sociometric status. Psychological Bulletin, 113(1), 99–128. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.113.1.99.
Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory, 2nd ed. McGraw-Hill.
Nylund-Gibson, K., & Choi, A. Y. (2018). Ten frequently asked questions about latent class analysis. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 4(4), 440–461. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000176
O’Mara, A. J., Marsh, H. W., Craven, R. G., & Debus, R. L. (2006). Do self-concept interventions make a difference? A synergistic blend of construct validation and meta-analysis. Educational Psychologist, 41, 181–206.
O’Reilly, C. (2013). Gifted education in Ireland. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 36, 97–118. doi:10.1177/0162353212470039
O’Reilly, C., Cross, T. L., & Cross, J. R. (2018, April). Preparing for psychological heterogeneity by identifying gifted students’ confidence in STEM and non-STEM disciplines. Presentation at the Wallace Research Symposium on Talent Development, Baltimore, MD.
Olszewski-Kubilius, P., Subotnik, R. F., & Worrell, F. C. (2015). Antecedent and concurrent psychosocial skills that support high levels of achievement within talent domains. High Ability Studies, 26(2), 195–210. https://doi.org/10.1080/13598139.2015.1095077
Olszewski-Kubilius, P., Subotnik, R. F., & Worrell, F. C. (2018). Talent development as a framework for gifted education: Implications for best practices and applications in schools. Prufrock Press.
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later personal adjustment: Are low- accepted children at risk? Psychological Bulletin, 102, 357–389.
Parker, W. D. (1997). An empirical typology of perfectionism in academically talented children. American Educational Research Journal, 34, 545 – 562.
Parker, W. D., & Mills, C. J. (1996). The incidence of perfectionism in gifted students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 40, 194 – 199.
Parkhurst, J. T., & Hopmeyer, A. (1998). Sociometric popularity and peer-perceived popularity two distinct dimensions of peer status. Journal of Early Adolescence, 18, 125–144. https://doi.org/10.1177/02724316980180 02001
Patterson, M. M., & Bigler, R. S. (2006). Preschool children’s attention to environmental messages about groups: Social categorization and the origins of intergroup bias. Child Development, 77(4), 847–860.
Peine, M., & Coleman, L. (2010). The phenomenon of waiting in class. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 34, 220-244.
Poteat, V. P., Horn, S. S., & Armstrong, P. I. (2017). Condoning discrimination: The effects of dominance and authoritarianism are moderated by different ways of reasoning about antigay discriminatory acts. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 20(6), 831–849. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430216638528
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., & Levin, S. (2006). Social dominance theory and the dynamics of intergroup relations: Taking stock and looking forward. European Review of Social Psychology, 17, 271-320. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463280601055772
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 741–763.
Prinstein, M. J., & La Greca, A. M. (2002). Peer crowd affiliation and internalizing distress in childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal follow-back study. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 12, 325–351.
Prinstein, M., & LaGreca, A. M. (2004). Childhood peer rejection and aggression as predictors of adolescent girls’ externalizing and health risk behaviors: A 6-year longitudinal study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 103–112.
Puckett, J. A., Zachary DuBois, L., McNeill, J. N., & Hanson, C. (2020). The association between social dominance orientation, critical consciousness, and gender minority stigma. Journal of Homosexuality, 67(8), 1081–1096. https://doi.org/10.1080/00918369.2019.1603493
Rammstedt, B., Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Borkenau, P. (2004). Resilients, overcon trollers, and undercontrollers: The replicability of the three personality prototypes across informants. European Journal of Personality, 18, 1–14.
Rayner, S. G. (2001). Aspects of the self as learner: Perception, concept, and esteem. In R. J. Riding & S. G. Rayner (Eds.), Self-perception (pp. 25–52). Ablex.
Rinn, A. N., Plucker, J. A., & Stocking, V. B. (2010). Fostering gifted students’ affective development: A look at the impact of academic self-concept. TEACHING Exceptional Children Plus, 6(4) Article 1. http://escholarship.bc.edu/education/tecplus/vol6/iss4/art1.
Robinson, N. (1996). Counseling agendas for gifted young people: A commentary. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 20, 128–137.
Rogers, K. B. (2015). The academic, socialization, and psychological effects of acceleration: Research synthesis. In N. Colangelo, J. L. VanTassel-Baska, A. Shoplik, & S. Assouline (Eds.), A nation empowered: Evidence trumps excuses holding back America’s brightest students (pp. 19-29). Belin Blank Center for Talent Development.
Rose, A., & Rudolph, K. (2006). A review of sex differences in peer relationship process: Potential trade-offs for emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 98–131.
Rose, T. (2016). The end of average: How we succeed in a world that values sameness (1st ed.). HarperOne.
Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Temperament, development, and personality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(4), 207-212.
Rudasill, K. M., Foust, R. C., & Callahan, C. M. (2007). The Social Coping Questionnaire: An examination of its structure with an American sample of gifted adolescents. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 30, 353–371.
Ruffman, T., Ruffman, C., Hill, S., Turunc, G., Park, N., Du, K., Hayhurst, J., Kang, J., Selçuk, B., Regenbrecht, H., Philipp, M. C., & Hunter, J. A. (2020). RWAc and SDOc: The measurement of right‐wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation in childhood. Social Development, 29(4), 1194–1214. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12438
Sak, U. (2004). A synthesis of research on psychological types of gifted adolescents. Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, 15, 70–79.
Sandstrom, M. J., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Eisenhower, A. (2003). Children’s appraisal of peer rejection experiences: Impact on social and emotional adjustment. Social Development, 12(4), 530–550. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00247
Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 609-612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009.
Schroder, H. S., Dawood, S., Yalch, M. M., Donnellan, M. B., & Moser, J. S. (2015). The role of implicit theories in mental health symptoms, emotion regulation, and hypothetical treatment choices in college students. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39(2), 120–139. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-014-9652-6
Schuler, P. A. (2000). Perfectionism and gifted adolescents. Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, 11(4), 183.
Shavelson, R. J., Hubner, J. J., & Stanton, G. C. (1976). Validation of construct interpretations. Review of Educational Research, 46, 407–441.
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression. Cambridge University Press.
Sidanius, J., Sinclair, S., & Pratto, F. (2006). Social dominance orientation, gender, and increasing educational exposure. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 1640-1653. https://doi.org/10.1111/ j.0021-9029.2006.00074.x
Siegle, D. (2007). Gifted children’s bill of rights. National Association for Gifted Children. https://www.nagc.org/resources-publications/resources-parents/gifted-ch…
Smith, M. M., Sherry, S. B., Rnic, K., Saklofske, D. H., Enns, M., & Gralnick, T. (2016). Are perfectionism dimensions vulnerability factors for depressive symptoms after controlling for neuroticism? A meta‐analysis of 10 longitudinal studies. European Journal of Personality, 30(2), 201–212. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2053
Smith, M. M., Sherry, S. B., Vidovic, V., Saklofske, D. H., Stoeber, J., & Benoit, A. (2019). Perfectionism and the Five-Factor Model of Personality: A meta-analytic review. Personality & Social Psychology Review, 23(4), 367–390. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868318814973
Soto, C. J., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2011). Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big-five domains and facets in a large cross-sectional sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 330–348.
Southern, W. T., & Jones, E. D. (2015). Types of acceleration: Dimensions and issues. In N. Colangelo, J. L. VanTassel-Baska, A. Shoplik, & S. Assouline (Eds.), A nation empowered: Evidence trumps excuses holding back America’s brightest students (pp. 9-18). Belin Blank Center for Talent Development.
Speirs Neumeister, K. L. (2004). Understanding the relationship between perfectionism and achievement motivation in gifted college students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 48(3), 219–231. https://doi.org/10.1177/001698620404800306
Speirs Neumeister, K. L (2016). Perfectionism in gifted children. In M. Neihart, S. I. Pfeiffer & T. L. Cross (Eds.), The social and emotional development of gifted children: What do we know? (2nd ed., pp. 29–40). Prufrock Press.
Stajkovic, A. D., Bandura, A., Locke, E. A., Lee, D., & Sergent, K. (2018). Test of three conceptual models of influence of the big five personality traits and self-efficacy on academic performance: A meta-analytic path-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 120, 238–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.08.014
Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Darling, N., Mounts, N. S., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1994). Over-time changes in adjustment and competence among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Development, 65, 754–77.
Stelmack, R. M. (1990). Biological bases of extraversion: Psychophysiological evidence. Journal of Personality, 58(1), 293–311. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1990.tb00917.x
Stoeber, J. (2014). Multidimensional perfectionism and the DSM-5 personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 64, 115–120.
Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: Approaches, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 295–319. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr10042
Stoeber, J., Otto, K., & Dalbert, C. (2009). Perfectionism and the Big Five: Conscientiousness predicts longitudinal increases in self-oriented perfectionism. Personality & Individual Differences, 47(4), 363–368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.04.004
Stricker, J., Buecker, S., Schneider, M., & Preckel, F. (2020). Intellectual giftedness and multidimensional perfectionism: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychology Review, 32(2), 391–414. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09504-1
Subotnik, R. F., Olszewski-Kubilius, P., & Worrell, F. C. (2011). Rethinking giftedness and gifted education: a proposed direction forward based on psychological science. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 12(1), 3-54. https:// doi.org/10.1177/1529100611418056
Swiatek, M. A. (1995). An empirical investigation of the social coping strategies used by gifted adolescents. Gifted Child Quarterly, 39(3), 154–161. https://doi.org/10.11 77/001698629503900305
Swiatek, M. A. (2001). Social coping among gifted high school students and its relationship to self-concept. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 30, 19–39. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005268704144
Swiatek, M. A. (2012). Social coping. In T. L. Cross & J. R. Cross (Eds.) Handbook for counselors serving students with gifts and talents (pp. 665-679). Prufrock Press.
Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 149–178.
Taub, A. (2022, March 31). Why Putin name-checked J. K. Rowling. New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2022/03/31/world/europe/putin-jk-rowling.html?s…
Terman, L. M. (1925). Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted children: Genetic studies of genius, Vol. 1. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Thompson, K. L., & Bundy, K. A. (1996). Social skills training for young adolescents: Cognitive and performance components. Adolescence, 31(123), 505.
Thomsen, L., Green, E. G. T., & Sidanius, J. (2008). We will hunt them down: How SDO and RWA fuels ethnic persecution of immigrants in fundamentally different ways. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1455-1464.
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 2(4), i-109.
Tomlinson, C. A. (1999). The differentiated classroom: Responding to the needs of all learners. Association for Supervising and Curriculum Development.
Tomlinson, C. A. (2005). Quality curriculum and instruction for highly able students. Theory Into Practice, 44(2), 160-166. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15430421tip4402_10
Van Leeuwen, K. G., Mervielde, I., Braet, C., & Bosmans, G. (2004). Child personality and parental behavior as moderators of problem behavior: Variable- and person-centered approaches. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1028–1046. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1028
Vuyk, M. A., Kerr, B. A. & Krieshok, T. S. (2016). From overexcitabilities to openness: Informing gifted education with psychological science. Gifted and Talented International, 31(1), 59-71. DOI: 10.1080/15332276.2016.1220796
Vuyk, M. A., Krieshok, T. S., & Kerr, B. A. (2016). Openness to experience rather than overexcitabilities: Call it like it is. Gifted Child Quarterly, 60, 192–211.
Wegner, D. M., & Vallacher, R. R. (1977). Implicit psychology. Oxford.
Whitley, B. E. (1999). Right-wing authoritarianism, social dominance orientation, and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(1), 126–134.
Whitley, B. E., & Kite, M. E. (Eds.). (2006). The psychology of prejudice and discrimination. Thomson Wadsworth.
Wilson, M. S. (2003). Social dominance and ethical ideology: The end justifies the means? The Journal of Social Psychology, 143, 549-558.
Yeager, D., & Dweck, C. (2012). Mindsets that promote resilience: When students believe that personal characteristics can be developed. Educational Psychologist, 47(4), 302–314. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2012.722805
Yeager D. S., Hanselman P., Walton G. M., Murray J. S., Crosnoe R., Muller C., Tipton E., Schneider B., Hulleman C. S., Hinojosa C. P., Paunesku D., Romero C., Flint K., Roberts A., Jill Trott, Iachan R., Buontempo J., Man Yang S., Carvalho C. M., Hahn P. R., Gopalan M., Mhatre P., Ferguson R., Duckworth A. L. & Dweck C. S. (2019). A national experiment reveals where a growth mindset improves achievement. Nature, 573, 364–369. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-019-1466-y
Zeidner, M., & Shani-Zinovich, I. (2011). Do academically gifted and non-gifted students differ on the big-five and adaptive status? Some recent data and conclusions. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 566–570.
Zell, A. L., Exline, J. J., & Lobel, M. (2020). Navigating the perils of outperforming others. In J. Suls, R. L. Collins, & L. Wheeler (Eds.), Social comparison, judgment, and behavior (pp. 280–308). Oxford University Press.
Zhang, Z. (2017). Gifted education in China. Cogent Education, 4, 1–12.
Ziegler, A., Fidelman, M., Reutlinger, M., Vialle, W., & Stoeger, H. (2010). Implicit personality theories on the modifiability and stability of the action repertoire as a meaningful framework for individual motivation: A cross-cultural study. High Ability Studies, 21(2), 147–163. https://doi.org/10.1080/13598139.2010.528924
Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., Gardner, A. A., Hawes, T., Masters, M. R., Waters, A. M., & Farrell, L. J. (2021). Rejection sensitivity and the development of social anxiety symptoms during adolescence: A five-year longitudinal study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 45(3), 204–215. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025421995921
Zimmerman, B. J., & Schunk, D. H. (2003). Albert Bandura: The scholar and his contributions to educational psychology. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Educational psychology: A century of contributions (pp. 431–457). Lawrence Erlbaum.
acceleration – any of myriad educational practices designed to move a student through curriculum already mastered, e.g., grade-skipping, curriculum compacting, dual enrollment, etc.
achievement goal orientation – a desire to learn for mastery of content or as a performance for others
adaptive/healthy perfectionism - perfectionistic attitudes that lead to excellence and positive psychological outcomes
agreeableness – considerate, kind, cooperative, prosocial behavior
Big-Fish-Little-Pond Effect – the experience of reduced self-concept when in the presence of others who perform better
cluster analysis – statistical technique for classifying cases (individuals) by multiple variables (characteristics) based on group similarity
conscientiousness – disciplined, organized, careful, diligent
differentiation – educational practice of modifying curriculum to meet the needs of learners of varying ability levels within the same classroom
downward comparison - perceiving onself as better than another person in characteristics or performance; part of social comparison theory
evaluative concerns – associated with socially prescribed perfectionism; a tendency to fear the negative evaluation of others in any effort that does not achieve perfection
extraversion – outgoing, talkative, active
five-factor model – personality model proposing five dimensions of openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism
fixed mindset – a belief that a human characteristic is an unchangeable entity within the person
group-based dominance – a belief that a hierarchical society should be maintained through a group’s aggressive assertion of dominance
growth mindset – a belief that a human characteristic can be changed, is malleable
homophily - biological tendency to be attracted to similar others
implicit theory – a theory one holds about the cause of events; beliefs that are implied rather than stated explicitly; may be unconscious beliefs
incremental – in implicit theory, a belief that change occurs in small, cumulative steps
invariance hypothesis – social dominance orientation is consistently higher among males than females
latent profile analysis – statistical technique for classifying cases (individuals) by multiple variables (characteristics) based on model fit
maladaptive/unhealthy perfectionism – perfectionistic attitudes that lead to negative psychological outcomes
neuroticism – moody, easily made anxious or distressed, fearful, emotionally unstable
openness to experience – includes intellectual curiosity, desire for variety and novelty, etc.
opposition to equality - a belief that a hierarchical society should be maintained due to a preference for inequality among groups
ostracism – rejection by others by being excluded or ignored
other-oriented perfectionism - expecting flawless performance by others
overcontroller – personality type high in neuroticism, low in extraversion; attempts to excessive control over responses to environment
perfectionism/perfectionistic attitudes – a tendency to hold the expectation of the self or others that one’s performance or characteristics should be flawless
personality – a set of enduring characteristics or patterns of behavior for an individual
positive striving – associated with self-oriented perfectionism; a tendency to try for perfection while maintaining a positive attitude toward the learning benefits of failure
resilient – in personality research, high in all personality dimensions; flexible, adaptive response to environment
self-beliefs – broadly defined understanding of the self, who one is, what they do well/poorly, what they like, etc.
self-concept – the beliefs one holds about their psychological and physical characteristics
self-efficacy – a perception of one’s ability to perform in a specific task or in general; component of motivation to try a task
self-oriented perfectionism – expecting flawless performance of the self
self-regulated learning – self-initiated monitoring and controlling of learning processes, such as studying, planning for assignments, etc.
sensitivity to being a target of threatening upward comparisons – concern that outperforming others may result in negative consequences, e.g., loss of friendship, physical harm
social cognitive beliefs – understanding and thought processes related to others
social comparison theory – all people compare themselves against others on any characteristic or performance; see upward/downward comparison
social coping – strategies one engages in to deal with the stigma of giftedness
social dominance orientation – a preference for groups in societies to be organized as egalitarian, with resources distributed equally among groups, or hierarchically, with one group holding disproportionate resources of social value
socially prescribed perfectionism – believing others expect flawless performance of oneself
stigma - the negative social attitude attached to a characteristic of an individual that may be regarded as a mental, physical, or social deficiency. a stigma implies social disapproval and can lead unfairly to discrimination against and exclusion of the individual. (apa dictionary)
stigma of giftedness paradigm – gifted students want normal social interactions, but once known as gifted will not be treated normally; will manage information to maximize social latitude
talent development – as a framework for gifted education, emphasizes identification of potential in early stages through offering opportunities to all students, increases challenge with maturity, focusing on development of talent in specific domains
undercontroller - personality type high in extraversion, low in agreeableness; poor control over responses to environment
upward comparison – perceiving another person as better than oneself in characteristics or performance; part of social comparison theory
well-being - a state of happiness and contentment, with low levels of distress, overall good physical and mental health and outlook, or good quality of life (from APA dictionary)
ACADHI – SCCLU2; high academics self-concept cluster
ANOVA - univariate analysis of variance
APA – American Psychological Association
BFI – Big Five Inventory personality scale
BFLPE – Big-Fish-Little-Pond Effect
CAT - Centre for Academic Talent program for students with test scores between 85th - 94th percentile
COVID-19 – Coronavirus disease caused by the SARS-CoV-2 virus
CTYG - Center for Talented Youth-Greece
CTYI – Centre for Talented Youth-Ireland or its programs for students with test scores at 95th percentile and above
ECHA – European Council for High Ability
EFA - Exploratory Factor Analysis
FFM – Five-Factor Model of personality
FFMCL1 – Moderate Resilients personality class; positive profile
FFMCL2 – Overcontrollers personality class; high neuroticism, low extraversion
FFMCL3 – Undercontrollers personality class; high extraversion, low agreeableness
FFMCL4 – High Resilients personality class; extremely positive profile
GBD – Group-Based Domination; see SDO
GENHI – overall high self-concept cluster
HAS – High Ability Studies Journal
INSPIRE - Innovation in Science Pursuit for Inspired Research program in Kolkata, India
IPT - Implicit Person Theory scale
JBNS - Jagadis Bose National Science Talent Search program in Kolkata, India
LPA - Latent Profile Analysis
LPA – Latent Profile Analysis
MSPSE - Multidimensional Scales of Perceived Self-Efficacy
NAGC – National Association for Gifted Children
OCEAN – Personality factors Openness to experience, Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, Neuroticism
OEQ – Opposition to Equality; see SDO
OES-A - Ostracism Experience Scale for Adolescents
OOP – other-oriented perfectionism
SCB – Social Cognitive Beliefs scale
SCCLU1 – GENHI self-concept cluster; overall high self-concept
SCCLU2 – ACADHI self-concept cluster; high academics self-concept
SCCLU3 – SCLOW self-concept cluster; overall low self-concept
SCCLU4 – SCMOD self-concept cluster; overall moderate self-concept
SCLOW – SCCLU3; overall low self-concept cluster
SCMOD – SCCLU4; overall moderate self-concept cluster
SCQ - Social Coping Questionnaire
SDO – Social Dominance Orientation
SECL1 – Pushovers self-efficacy class
SECL2 – Insecure self-efficacy class
SECL3 – Need a Boost self-efficacy class
SECL4 – Confident Majority self-efficacy class
SECL5 – Superstars self-efficacy class
SECL6 – Confident Pushovers self-efficacy class
SECLn – Self-Efficacy Class number
SEGSS – Social Experience of Gifted Students Scale
SEGSS – Social Experience of Gifted Students Scale
SGP - Stigma of Giftedness Paradigm
SOP – self-oriented perfectionism
SPP – socially prescribed perfectionism
SRA – Society for Research on Adolescence
SRCD – Society for Research in Child Development
STTUC - sensitivity to being a target of threatening upward comparison